SIFT, Science vs Pseudoscience, and Research Methods Notes
- SIFT stands for Stop, Investigate, Find, Trace.
- Stop: pause to check your emotions before reacting to information. Example: TikTok or social media content about sensitive topics (e.g., school shootings) can provoke strong emotions; important to pause and reassess before forming judgments.
- Investigate: identify the source and its background.
- Ask: Who is the source? Is it from a scientific journal, an expert, or another outlet?
- Consider the background of the expert (method of authority) and whether not all experts are truly experts.
- Assess the agenda or motives of the source.
- Do not rely on a single source.
- Find: locate the best information on the topic from multiple trusted sources.
- Look for credible outlets, journals, and corroborating evidence.
- Trace: trace the claims back to their original sources.
- Check for original peer-reviewed content (e.g., in prestigious journals such as JAMA).
- Trace the quotes or claims to the primary material to verify accuracy.
- Example applied to a TikTok claim about Disney lowering the drinking age to 18 (years ago):
- Stop: manage emotional reaction to the claim.
- Investigate: identify Mousetrap News as the source; note it provided no court filings or evidence; the piece had a clunky write-up and no byline.
- Find: compare with other outlets (Yahoo News, MSN, etc.); check for legitimate reporting.
- Trace: Mousetrap News labeled itself as “real Disney news” but that is a 100% fake site; verify the site’s about page and other sources; check whether other outlets carried the story; no credible peer-reviewed article found.
- Additional credibility check tips:
- Look for a byline and the author’s credentials.
- Check the article’s evidence, such as court filings, official documents, or data.
- Seek multiple sources to confirm the claim.
- Be wary of articles lacking verifiable sources or journalistic quality (punctuation errors, no author, no corroboration).
- Distinguishing myth vs fact example:
- Myth: “your sight deteriorates if you read in the dark or on a monitor.”
- Fact: eyes get tired quickly, recover after rest; the difference is about fatigue, not permanent deterioration.
- Purpose of SIFT: to cultivate critical evaluation of information, especially in an age of easy dissemination on social media and diverse news outlets.
Science vs Pseudoscience
- Pseudoscience: appears scientific but is often deceptive.
- Uses scientific terms to give an illusion of legitimacy.
- Hypotheses are often unverifiable and based on anecdotal evidence.
- Tends to be irrefutable and not easily testable against disconfirming evidence.
- Relies on selective evidence and may deploy jargon to convince lay audiences.
- Demonstrates confirmation bias and belief perseverance; uses new jargon to seem credible.
- Science: evidence-based, testable, and self-correcting.
- Hypotheses are solvable and testable, and can be directly observed with instruments.
- Questions asked are solvable and testable through empirical methods.
- Hypotheses are empirical, systematic, falsifiable, and open to public verification.
- Public verification: findings are shared with the scientific community through journals and conferences; others can critique and replicate.
- Replication is central to verification and self-correction; findings may be preregistered and data openly shared.
- Key characteristics of science (four):
- Empirical: based on observable data.
- Systematic: bias-minimized, structured, and repeatable methods.
- Falsifiable: capable of being proven false.
- Public verification: results subjected to peer review and replication.
- Important processes in science:
- Preregistration: researchers publicly declare hypotheses and analysis plans before data collection; helps prevent HARKing (hypothesizing after results are known).
- Replication: independent repetition of studies to verify results.
- Distinction between fraud (deliberate deception) and ordinary errors/mistakes; science is self-correcting but requires ongoing verification.
- Post hoc vs a priori hypotheses:
- A priori: predictions made before data are collected; stronger evidentiary value.
- Post hoc: hypotheses generated after examining the data; higher risk of type I error.
- Practical implication: in the real world, pseudoscience can masquerade as science; science emphasizes testability, openness, and falsifiability.
The Four Primary Goals of Science
- Describe behavior: describe what is observed in a given situation using systematic observations.
- Predict behavior: forecast when and under what conditions a behavior will occur; involves models and analyses (e.g., regression) to forecast outcomes.
- Explain (determine causes of) behavior: true experiments enable causal inferences due to randomization, manipulation of variables, and control of extraneous factors.
- True experiments require:
- Random assignment to conditions.
- Manipulation of the independent variable.
- Control over confounding variables.
- Temporal precedence: the cause occurs before the effect.
- Temporal precedence: the time order must be such that the cause precedes the effect, often stated as t(cause) < t(effect).
- Covariation/correlation: variables must covary to support potential causal links, but covariation alone does not prove causation.
- Directionality and third-variable problems: correlational designs cannot establish causality due to potential reverse causation and confounds.
- Modify/Change behavior: research can aim to alter behavior (applied aims) based on underlying theories.
- Distinctions within the reasoning framework:
- A priori predictions are made before data collection; post hoc explanations are generated after data analysis.
- A key distinction is between causal inference (need true experiments) and correlational findings (cannot establish causality without temporal precedence and control).
- What makes a strong hypothesis (four criteria):
- Empirical: observable and measurable.
- Systematic: conducted under controlled and unbiased procedures.
- Falsifiable: possible to be proven false.
- Public verification: results shared for scrutiny and replication.
- Important statistical concepts touched in this section:
- Type I error: rejecting a true null hypothesis.
- Defined as \alpha = P(\text{reject } H0 | H0 \text{ is true}).
- Null hypothesis: a statement of no effect or no difference to be tested against.
- Post hoc analyses increase the risk of Type I error if not properly controlled.
Experimental vs Nonexperimental Methods; Basic vs Applied Research
- Nonexperimental (descriptive) research: describes behavior but does not explain why or how; methods include:
- Observational (naturalistic and laboratory settings): watch and record behavior.
- Case studies: intensive examination of a single case or a few individuals.
- Surveys: questionnaires and interviews to gather broad data.
- Historical/archival research: examining past records to relate to current events.
- Correlational research: examines relationships between variables but cannot prove causation.
- Qualitative vs quantitative approaches; mixed methods combine both.
- Experimental research: true experiments that establish cause-effect relationships through manipulation and control; includes random assignment and manipulation of the independent variable.
- Basic (pure) vs Applied research:
- Basic research seeks to build and refine knowledge and theories, sometimes with no immediate practical application (e.g., theoretical issues in cognitive psychology).
- Applied research seeks solutions to real-world problems and often involves program evaluation and immediate applicability.
- These two can overlap and inform each other; feedback between basic findings and applied contexts often leads to new hypotheses and revised theories.
- Examples discussed in class:
- Classical conditioning (Pavlov) discovered serendipitously during digestion studies; led to therapies like systematic desensitization.
- Practical example: addressing practical problems like designing dorms and dining halls to improve efficiency and student experience (human factors angle).
- Applied research example: program evaluation in education and professional development (workshops, institutes).
- The two-arrow concept in slides (inform each other): basic research informs applied work and applied findings can refine or challenge theories in basic research.
- Research design considerations mentioned:
- Selection bias and the importance of random assignment to avoid biased groups.
- Quasi-experiments: preexisting groups where random assignment is not possible; can imply causation but with limitations.
- Mixed designs: combining quasi-manipulated and actual manipulated variables.
- Temporal considerations and control of extraneous factors are crucial for drawing conclusions about causality.
- The research process and real-world constraints:
- Idea generation often comes from prior literature, theory, observation, and personal experiences.
- Reading the literature, examining references, and identifying gaps are essential steps.
- Theories provide a framework to generate hypotheses and predictions; testing against competing theories is common (e.g., false memory theories: fuzzy-trace theory vs. source-monitoring theory).
- Practical considerations for applied settings:
- Program evaluation in education and organizational settings.
- Interdisciplinary areas such as human factors combine cognitive science and engineering to improve system design.
- Real-world problems can drive research questions and experimental designs.
Primary vs Secondary Sources
- Primary sources: the original or firsthand accounts of observations and results.
- Examples: journal articles, theses and dissertations, conference papers/posters, interview transcripts, raw data (e.g., recorded and transcribed interviews), original artifacts.
- Characteristics: firsthand reports and raw observations directly from the researcher(s).
- Secondary sources: secondhand reports that discuss or summarize primary sources.
- Examples: textbooks, review papers, meta-analyses, popular articles that synthesize others’ research.
- Risks: misinterpretation, out-of-context quotations, incomplete representations of original work.
- Practical guidance:
- When in doubt, seek the primary source to verify details.
- Be cautious with secondary sources that may omit nuances or misrepresent findings.
- Meta-analyses synthesize data across studies but still rely on the quality of the included primary studies.
- News outlets and popular media may not provide complete methodological details; verify with primary sources.
- Special notes:
- The file-drawer problem: some studies with null results remain unpublished; this affects the completeness of the literature and calls for including null results when possible (e.g., journals dedicated to null findings).
- The Journal of Null Findings is an example of a venue that highlights null results to address publication bias.
Research Databases and Literature Searching (Berry Library Context)
- Library resources and search tools mentioned:
- Berry library website: www.berry.edu/library
- Journal locator, online catalog, research videos, research guide, and access to research databases (left sidebar).
- WorldCat as a broad search tool for articles, books, and videos.
- Focus on psychology databases under Psych Articles / PsycArticles (represented as PsycArticles and PsychArticles in the lecture).
- Practical search workflow:
- Start with the research databases section for psychology (P). Examples include Psych articles and the PsycArticles databases.
- Use Advanced Search rather than Basic Search for more control over search fields and operators.
- Begin with a general topic idea and generate keywords or subject terms to use in searches.
- Consider starting with a secondary source to gain a broad overview and then drill down to primary sources.
- Build a literature map by tracing back to classic foundational papers and then follow newer papers that cite them to see how lines of thought have evolved.
- Use multiple databases to ensure coverage and to locate both classic and current work.
- The tree-branch analogy: classic foundational work forms the trunk; recent work forms the tips, highlighting how research has branched and evolved.
- Practical tips for students new to psychology literature searching:
- Don’t rely solely on Google or AI-generated resources; use library databases for peer-reviewed and credible sources.
- When you locate a relevant article, check its references for additional primary sources.
- For assignments, you may be asked to annotate several articles and discuss their variables and relationships; these are usually not descriptive-only but aim to identify relationships and possible hypotheses.
- The first writing assignment will build on mastering these sources and the ability to distinguish primary from secondary literature.
- Closing note on research workflow:
- Start with a topic you find interesting, locate several articles, extract key variables, examine how they relate, and identify potential hypotheses and study designs for future work.
- The process emphasizes critical thinking, careful sourcing, and an understanding that theory and data guide inquiry—often in a dynamic, iterative loop between basic and applied questions.