Focus on inductive and causal arguments
Three important kinds of arguments:
Inductive Generalizations: Lead to general knowledge.
Causal Arguments: Discuss causes and effects.
Applications: Apply general knowledge to specific cases.
Involves concluding about a whole category based on information about part of it.
Example: Expecting a crow to be black based on previous observations.
Everyday examples:
Expecting a bus to be late if it has often been so.
Delaying tomato planting until late May due to past cold nights.
Public Opinion Polls:
Polling firms use samples to estimate the opinions of a population, e.g. voting results in the 2018 US elections.
Draw conclusions about causes based on premises about items and their connections.
Example: Lung cancer is more prevalent in smokers, indicating smoking contributes to lung cancer.
Details are essential for a strong causal argument:
Examining other explanations, e.g., mechanical causes in aircraft crashes.
Use of general premises about populations to draw conclusions about specific cases.
Example:
Knowing few men live past 100 leads to not expecting to live that long.
Low lung cancer rates among nonsmokers reduce worry for non-smokers.
"X percent of items of kind A have property P.
Conclusion: Approximately X percent of all A have property P."
Example: Polling in Ottawa regarding urban boundaries.
Sample: Items observed.
Population: The entire collection the conclusion is about.
Valid inductive generalizations occur when the sample equals the population.
Rare; usually samples are part of the population.
Essential criteria: Quality of data, representativeness of the sample, sample size.
Quality of Data:
Consider how data was collected, methods, and reliability.
Assess potential bias and conflicts of interest.
Representativeness of the Sample:
Ensure it reflects the population.
Random sampling ideal but difficult.
Bias examples:
Street polls are led by passerby responses, missing many demographics.
Phone and internet surveys exclude those without phones or internet access.
Sample Size:
Small samples less likely to represent the population accurately.
Samples around 2000 are considered adequate in Canada.
Polls come with a margin of error and confidence measure.
Understanding these reduces misinterpretation of results.
Predictable World Bias: The incorrect belief that patterns exist without evidence.
Confirmation Bias: Noticing data that supports preconceptions while ignoring contrary data.
Availability Bias: Using easily obtained data without ensuring its comprehensive validity.
Hasty Generalization: Jumping to conclusions based on small data.
Apriorism: Drawing conclusions without examining data.
Revisability: An inductive conclusion may change with more data.
COVID-19 in the Philippines vs. Hong Kong:
Weak argument due to sample size and varying testing rates.
North vs. South Korea COVID-19 Comparisons:
Reliable data from North Korea is lacking; conclusion not justified.
U.S. Senators' Stock Sales:
Insufficient sample size to generalize about all politicians.
Italy vs. Germany COVID-19 Mortality Rates:
Unequal testing regimes undermine the comparison accuracy.
Importance of careful evaluation of problems in inductive reasoning and data quality.
Reminder to use critical thinking when interpreting polls and data.