Last-minute Review: Correlation, Experimentation, and Brain Micro Level
Correlation basics
Positive correlation: as one variable increases, the other tends to increase. Example: SAT scores and college grades tend to move together.
Actual real-world strength: about r \approx 0.2 \text{ to } 0.25; not a strong predictor on its own.
Negative correlation: as one variable increases, the other tends to decrease (e.g., higher daily alcohol intake associated with lower grades).
Zero correlation: no predictable relationship (e.g., hand size vs mass scores when you remove certain outliers).
Perfect correlation: all data points lie exactly on a straight line (rare in reality).
Correlation vs causation
Correlation does not imply causation; third variables can influence both.
Classic example: ice cream sales and deaths in different regions rise together in hot weather; heat is a third variable affecting both.
Smoking and cancer: correlation exists, but establishing causation requires experiments.
Experimentation vs correlation:
Correlation alone cannot establish causality.
Experiments isolate A causing B by manipulating A (IV) and measuring B (DV) while controlling other factors.
No causation without experimentation.
Experimental design essentials
Random assignment vs random sampling:
Random assignment: place participants into conditions so each has an equal chance of being in any condition (great equalizer).
Random sampling: how you select participants for the study (not the focus here).
Variables:
IV (independent variable): what the researcher manipulates to see what happens.
DV (dependent variable): what is measured to assess the effect.
Confounds: unwanted variables that can threaten interpretation and make results uninterpretable.
Control group: a baseline condition not receiving the experimental manipulation (or receiving placebo) to isolate the effect of the IV.
Placebo effect and deception: sometimes deception is used to preserve study validity; ethics require debriefing and often informed consent.
Ethical safeguards:
IRB/review boards to protect participants’ rights.
Informed consent and debriefing after the study.
Examples:
Antidepressant trial: depressed participants assessed at Time 1, receive the drug, reassessed at Time 2; multiple potential confounds (seasonal effects, therapy, placebo).
Beer goggles experiment: rating attractiveness before and after drinking; need a control (non-drinking) condition to separate drinking effects from other factors like lighting, mood, desperation, or ratings by different people.
Key issues to watch for: ensuring the same people are rated at Time 1 and Time 2; avoiding non-random assignment when comparing groups (e.g., drinkers vs non-drinkers) because it introduces confounds.
Study design reminders:
If groups differ at baseline, you cannot attribute end-of-study differences to the manipulation alone.
Random assignment helps balance group characteristics.
Some experiments involve deception; always consider whether the ends justify the means and ensure participant protection.
IVs, DVs, and confounds (quick recap)
IV = what you manipulate; DV = what you measure.
Confounds = other variables that could explain observed effects.
A well-designed experiment uses random assignment and a proper control condition to minimize confounds.
Building blocks of the brain: micro level (neuroscience basics)
Neurons are the basic building blocks of the brain and nervous system; they process and transmit information.
Neuron structure:
Dendrites: receive information from other neurons (receptors).
Cell body (soma): contains the nucleus; processes incoming information.
Nucleus: stores genetic material; part of the cell’s decision-making core.
Axon: transmits information away from the cell body to other neurons.
Axon terminal: end of the axon where signals are transmitted to the next neuron.
Signal transmission basics:
Resting potential: the neuron’s baseline electrical state, roughly -70\ \text{mV} outside and inside the neuron with a gradient of ions.
Ion gradients and gates: ions (sodium, potassium) move through channels; gates open in response to signals.
Threshold and action potential: if inputs push the neuron to threshold, an all-or-nothing spike (action potential) travels along the axon.
Propagation mechanism: once threshold is reached, ion channels open in sequence, creating a wave of depolarization that travels down the axon; ions are pumped back to maintain resting potential after the spike.
Conceptual takeaway: neurons carry and communicate information via electrical signals (action potentials) and chemical signaling at synapses; this supports information processing in the brain.
Quick reference concepts (syntax-friendly)
Correlation coefficient: r \approx 0.2 \text{ to } 0.25 is a weak positive relationship.
Resting potential: -70\ \text{mV}.
Action potential: an all-or-nothing electrical impulse along the axon.
Independent variable (IV): manipulated by the researcher.
Dependent variable (DV): measured to see effect of the IV.
Confound: a variable that undermines interpretability of results.
Control group: baseline condition to compare against the experimental manipulation.
Deception and ethics: sometimes used in social psychology; requires debriefing and protection of participants.
Random assignment: key method to create equivalent groups and support causal conclusions.
IRB and informed consent: protections for participants in research.
Macro vs micro (preview)
Micro: focus on neurons and cellular processes (this section).
Macro: to be covered later, focusing on systems and larger brain structures and networks.