knowt logo

The Prototype Approach

Empirical Evidence

  • Eleanor Rosch (1975)

    • Participants were shown lists of words from the same category (e.g., fruit)

    • Rated each example on 7-point scale indicating how "good" a member of the category it was

    • Results: apples & oranges were "best" examples of this category

  • Rosch & Mervis (1975)

    • Group 1 provided more prototype ratings for words in several categories (vehicles, vegetables, clothing)

    • Group 2 listed attributes of each item

    • Results: significant correlations between prototype ratings and shared attributes

      • Care shares many attributes with other vehicles

      • Raft shares few attributes with other vehicles

Typicality Effect

  • The more features a member possess, the more (proto)
    typical it is

  • Some things are better examples of a concept than others

  • A songbird is a more typical bird than an ostrich

  • High prototypicality: category member closely resembles
    category prototype

    • "Typical member"

    • For category "bird' = robin

  • Low prototypicality: category member does not closely resemble category prototype

    • "Non-typical" member

    • For category "bird" = penguin

  • Prototypical objects are processed preferentially

  • Highly prototypical objects judged more rapidly

    • Sentence verification technique

  • Rosch (1973)

    • Participants: children, adults

      • Questions such as

        • Is an apple a fruit? (central member of category)

        • Is a fig a fruit? (peripheral member of category)

    • DV: Response Times

      • Results

        • Adults faster than children overall

        • Faster responses to central than peripheral members

  • Smith, Shoben & Rips (1974)

    • Typicality effect: Sentences about (proto)typical instances were verified faster than sentences about less (proto)typical instances

  • Mervis, Catlin, & Rosch (1976)

    • Instances rated as highly typical are more often named as examples of the category

      • BIRD category

      • ROBIN is rated as more typical than penguin

    • ROBIN is more likely than penguin to be given as an example of bird

 

Rosch 1975, The Typicality Effect

  • Highly typical influences are used as cognitive reference points

  • Presented pairs of stimuli

    • One was a prototype of category (e.g., circle)

    • Other was similar but not prototype (e.g., eclipse)

  • People were more likely to say

    • Eclipse is essentially a circle than a circle is essentially an eclipse

 

Prototype Approach - Limitations

  • When the task of categorising things is set by how well it meets a goal - it is no longer about typicality being defined by family resemblance

  • Prototypes do not always represent the features of most members of the category

    • For example, Lynch et al. (2000) had participants provide typicality ratings for different trees - instead of trees representing the most common height being identified, the tallest trees were!

      • Maybe representing an 'ideal' tree

  • Familiarity may overrule prototype matching

  • Abstract concepts

    • Justice, peace, etc

  • Ad hoc categories

     

Exemplar Approach

  • Concept is represented by multiple examples (rather than a single prototype)

  • Examples are actual category members (not abstract averages)

  • To categorise, compare the new item to stored examples

  • Similar to prototype view

    • Representing a category is not defining it

  • Different: representation is not abstract

    • Descriptions of specific examples

  • The more similar a specific exemplar is to a known category member, the faster it was be categorised (family resemblance effect)

 

Benefits of the Exemplar Approach

  • Explains peoples' inability to state necessary & sufficient features. How?

    • There are none

  • Explains difficulty categorising atypical instances. How?

    • They are too dissimilar to previously stored instances

  • Explains Typicality effects. How?

    • Numerous similar stored instances make typical instances easier (faster) to classify

 

Problems with the Exemplar Approach

  • Does not specify…

    • Which instances will / will not be stored as exemplars

    • How different exemplars are called to mind at time of categorisation

    • How are concepts organised?

  • Is truth somewhere in the middle?

    • Exemplars may work best for small categories

    • Prototypes may work best for larger categories

  • These two approaches may not be mutually exclusive, rather as two ends of an abstraction continuum

 

 

The Prototype Approach

Empirical Evidence

  • Eleanor Rosch (1975)

    • Participants were shown lists of words from the same category (e.g., fruit)

    • Rated each example on 7-point scale indicating how "good" a member of the category it was

    • Results: apples & oranges were "best" examples of this category

  • Rosch & Mervis (1975)

    • Group 1 provided more prototype ratings for words in several categories (vehicles, vegetables, clothing)

    • Group 2 listed attributes of each item

    • Results: significant correlations between prototype ratings and shared attributes

      • Care shares many attributes with other vehicles

      • Raft shares few attributes with other vehicles

Typicality Effect

  • The more features a member possess, the more (proto)
    typical it is

  • Some things are better examples of a concept than others

  • A songbird is a more typical bird than an ostrich

  • High prototypicality: category member closely resembles
    category prototype

    • "Typical member"

    • For category "bird' = robin

  • Low prototypicality: category member does not closely resemble category prototype

    • "Non-typical" member

    • For category "bird" = penguin

  • Prototypical objects are processed preferentially

  • Highly prototypical objects judged more rapidly

    • Sentence verification technique

  • Rosch (1973)

    • Participants: children, adults

      • Questions such as

        • Is an apple a fruit? (central member of category)

        • Is a fig a fruit? (peripheral member of category)

    • DV: Response Times

      • Results

        • Adults faster than children overall

        • Faster responses to central than peripheral members

  • Smith, Shoben & Rips (1974)

    • Typicality effect: Sentences about (proto)typical instances were verified faster than sentences about less (proto)typical instances

  • Mervis, Catlin, & Rosch (1976)

    • Instances rated as highly typical are more often named as examples of the category

      • BIRD category

      • ROBIN is rated as more typical than penguin

    • ROBIN is more likely than penguin to be given as an example of bird

 

Rosch 1975, The Typicality Effect

  • Highly typical influences are used as cognitive reference points

  • Presented pairs of stimuli

    • One was a prototype of category (e.g., circle)

    • Other was similar but not prototype (e.g., eclipse)

  • People were more likely to say

    • Eclipse is essentially a circle than a circle is essentially an eclipse

 

Prototype Approach - Limitations

  • When the task of categorising things is set by how well it meets a goal - it is no longer about typicality being defined by family resemblance

  • Prototypes do not always represent the features of most members of the category

    • For example, Lynch et al. (2000) had participants provide typicality ratings for different trees - instead of trees representing the most common height being identified, the tallest trees were!

      • Maybe representing an 'ideal' tree

  • Familiarity may overrule prototype matching

  • Abstract concepts

    • Justice, peace, etc

  • Ad hoc categories

     

Exemplar Approach

  • Concept is represented by multiple examples (rather than a single prototype)

  • Examples are actual category members (not abstract averages)

  • To categorise, compare the new item to stored examples

  • Similar to prototype view

    • Representing a category is not defining it

  • Different: representation is not abstract

    • Descriptions of specific examples

  • The more similar a specific exemplar is to a known category member, the faster it was be categorised (family resemblance effect)

 

Benefits of the Exemplar Approach

  • Explains peoples' inability to state necessary & sufficient features. How?

    • There are none

  • Explains difficulty categorising atypical instances. How?

    • They are too dissimilar to previously stored instances

  • Explains Typicality effects. How?

    • Numerous similar stored instances make typical instances easier (faster) to classify

 

Problems with the Exemplar Approach

  • Does not specify…

    • Which instances will / will not be stored as exemplars

    • How different exemplars are called to mind at time of categorisation

    • How are concepts organised?

  • Is truth somewhere in the middle?

    • Exemplars may work best for small categories

    • Prototypes may work best for larger categories

  • These two approaches may not be mutually exclusive, rather as two ends of an abstraction continuum

 

 

robot