Cognitive Tool box
We have some fundamental, automatic cognitive abilities, such as (Kahneman, 2003):
The ability to judge similarities between objects or events;
the ability to recognize a previously experienced situation or individual;
The ability of retrieve additional information about an object or situation once it has been recognized;
The ability to “see” casual relationships between events
Two heuristics, two cognitive capacities
Availability heuristic → memory retrieval
Representativeness heuristic → similarity assessment
Representativeness is an assessment of the degree of correspondence between a sample and a population, an instance and a category, an act and an actor or, more generally, between an outcome and a model
Representativeness heuristic
People rely on representativeness heuristics in which probabilities are evaluated by the degree to which A is representative of B (that is, by the degree to which A resembles B)
E.g when A is highly representative of B, the probability that A originates from B is judged to be high
When A is not similar to B, the probability that A originates from B is judged to be low
Conjunction Fallacy
The conjunction of two events is ranked to be more likely than one or both of the conjuncts
The violation of the conjunction rule is called the conjunction fallacy
Conjunction fallacy: Mathematical Explanation
Let A = “Linda is a bank teller”
Let B = “Linda is active in the feminist movement”
The probability of A and B (the conjunction) is denoted as P(A∩B) according to the conjunction rule in probability
P(A∩B) ≤ P(A)
This means that the probability of two events (A and B) occurring together cannot be higher than the probability of either one occurring alone
For instance:
Let P(A) = 0.4 (40% chance Linda is a bank teller)
Let P(B) = 0.6 (60% chance Linda is active in the feminist movement)
then , the maximum possible value of P(A∩B) (if A and B were independent) would be:
P(A∩B) = P(A) x P(B) = 0.4 x 0.6 = 0.24
The subjects ranked the outcomes by the degree to which Bill (or Linda) matched the respective stereotypes
The correlation between the mean ranks of probability and representativeness was .96 for Bill and .98 for Linda
“Transparent Version”
A group of 142 undergraduates at UBC were asked to check which of two alternatives was more probable:
Linda is a bank teller (B)
Linda is a bank teller and is active in the feminist movement (B&F)
The order of alternative was inverted for one half of the subjects, but this manipulation had no effect
Overall 85% of respondents indicated that B&F was more probable than B, in a violation of the conjunction rule
“Additional explanation” version
Another group of UBC udnergraduates was asked to indicate which of the following two arguments they found more convincing:
Argument #1: Linda is more likely to be a bank teller thank she is to be a feminist bank teller, because every feminist bank teller is a bank teller, but some women bank tellers are not feminists, and Linda could be one of them
Argument #2: Linda is more likely to be a feminist bank teller than she is likely to be a bank teller, because she resembles an active feminist more than she resembles a bank teller
The majority of subjects (65%, n = 58) chose the invalid resemblance argument (Argument 2) over the valid extensional argument (Argument 1)
Conjunction Fallacy
Naive subject do not spontaneously treat the conjunction rule as decisive
Naive subjects generally endorse the conjunction rule in the abstract, but their application of this rule to the Linda problem is blocked by the compelling impression that B&F is mro representative of her than B is
Conjunction Fallacy
103 physicians were given problems of the following type:
A 55 year old woman had pulmonary embolism (blood clots in the lung) documented angiographically 10 days after a cholecystectomy. Please rank the following in terms of the probability that they will be among the conditions experienced by the patient (use 1 for most likely and 6 for the least likely) Naturally the patient could experience more than one of these conditions
Dyspnea (shortness of breath and hemiparesis (partial paralysis)
Calf pain
Pleuritic chest pain
Syncope and tachycardia
Hemipareisis
Hemoptysis
91% of physicians violated the conjunction rule
Conjunction fallacy is robust to:
Transparency
Additional explanation
Statement of extension of (whether or not)
Expertise
Conjunction fallacy among children
In a simpler form with children participants (Agnoli, 1991)
In summer at the beach, are there more women or more tanned women?
Does the mailman put more letters or more pieces of mail in your mailbox?
Conjunction “fallacy”?
Gigerenzer et al → “adaptive toolbox”
Content-blind norms
E.g semantic ambiguity of “AND” operator (union vs intersection)
“We invited friends and colleagues to the party”
Probability vs frequency formats
The representativeness heuristic overrides the factors that should affect judgment
Insensitivity to prior probability of outcomes (base-rates)
A certain town has 2 hospitals
Large hospital: About 45 babies are born each day
Small hospital: About 15 babies are born each day
In each hospital about 50% of babies are boys, However, the exact percentage varies from day to day some days it might be higher, and some days lower
Insensitivity to prior probability of outcomes (base-rates)
Insensitivity to sample size
Misconception of chance
Example: Coin Flip → HTHTTH to be more likely than HHHTTT
Conjunction fallacy & Prejudice
Gervais, Shariff and Morenzayan (2011): Do you believe in atheists? Distrust is central to anti-atheist prejudice