Author: George Yule
Speculation about the beginnings of language:
Early humans (males and females) might have used musical notes and rhythm to express love before developing articulate language (Darwin, 1871).
Darwin suggests that musical ability predates language, an insight that challenges typical perceptions of prehistoric humans.
Despite this speculation, there is no direct evidence on how language originated.
Production of sounds appears to be traced back to an ancient part of the brain shared with vertebrates.
Spoken language likely developed between 100,000 and 50,000 years ago, well before written language, which appeared about 5,000 years ago.
Absence of artifacts or physical evidence from earlier periods means that speculations about the origins continue.[27]
Biblical Traditions:
Genesis describes God creating Adam and assigning names to creatures.
Hindu traditions ascribe the origin of language to Sarasvati, the goddess of knowledge.
Many religions believe in a divine source for language.
Early experiments aimed to rediscover this original divine language have yielded conflicting results.
Historical Experiment:
Herodotus recounts Egyptian Pharaoh Psammetichus's experiment with isolated infants, who spoke the Phrygian word "bekos" (meaning "bread").
The conclusion drawn was that Phrygian must be the original language, despite other explanations for the children's utterances.
King James IV's experiment around 1500 suggested children spoke Hebrew after isolation.
Evidence from other isolation cases (like Victor or Genie) shows that children without exposure to language fail to develop any.
This suggests that spontaneous language development from silence isn’t feasible.
Biblical account of Babel raises questions about reconstructing any original language.
The theory proposes that early humans associated natural sounds with their meanings.
Human auditory capabilities exist in utero, culminating in sound recognition abilities.
Jespersen's "bow-wow" theory suggests early humans imitated these sounds to create primitive words.
Primitive words linked to imitations of sounds from the environment (Caw-Caw, Coo-Coo).
Modern examples: words like splash, bang, rattle, buzz.
Onomatopoeia demonstrates the vocalization derived from imitative sounds.
This theory struggles to explain how abstract concepts could have been named.
Proposed that speech evolved from instinctive emotional sounds (e.g., Ouch!).
Criticism: These sounds are generally produced on inhalation, contrasting with normal speech production which occurs during exhalation.
Jespersen's "yo-he-ho" theory suggests language arose from sounds made during communal physical effort.
Communication within groups would be necessary for coordination.
While social interactions require language, it does not clarify the origins of sound production.
Emphasizes anatomical features (like an upright posture) that evolved to support speech production.
Comparative anatomy between humans and primates highlights significant skull and vocal tract differences.
Human teeth are structured differently to facilitate speech sounds.
Upright teeth are better for producing sounds like f and v.
Human lips possess intricate muscles for creating versatile sounds( p, b, m).
Unique positioning of human larynx enhances vocal capabilities, creating a resonating pharynx.
This phylogenetic change resulted in increased vocal range, despite the choking risk it introduces.
Physical adaptations for speech may correspond with manual skills, suggesting gestures precursored language.
Tool-making shows evidence of cognitive development linked with speech.
The brain’s lateralization connects functions of speech and tool-making.
Complex vocalization involves specific brain regions closely related to motor functions, indicating evolutionary connections between speech and tool use.
Infants exhibit rapid developmental changes like brain growth and larynx descent, crucial for language acquisition.
Some propose innate language capacity, possibly linked to a genetic mutation unique to humans.
Phonetics establishes a framework for analyzing speech sounds.
The International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA).
Different branches of phonetics: articulatory, acoustic, and auditory.
Distinction based on vocal cord positioning:
Voiceless: vocal folds apart, unimpeded airflow.
Voiced: vocal folds together, causing vibration.
Examples demonstrating the difference with practical exercises.
Consonant sounds are categorized by their places of articulation within the oral cavity.
Visual representation via anatomical diagrams can clarify this concept.
Common sounds and their phonetic symbols explained with practical examples.
Voiced and voiceless sounds identified and contrasted.
Details on how sounds are produced:
Stops: Airflow temporarily blocked.
Fricatives: Airflow nearly blocked creating friction.
Affricates: Combines stops and fricatives for complex sounds.
Discusses nasals and liquids and their articulation.
Explores glottal stops and flaps common in casual speech.
These sounds often illustrate variations in pronunciation within English, specifically American dialects.
Vowel production differs from consonants; they require a more open vocal tract.
Classification by tongue position helps define vowel sounds.
Combination of vowel sounds that shift position during articulation.
Practical examples illustrate common diphthongs.
Discusses how vowel pronunciation varies between different English accents.
Notes how certain vowel sounds can be produced more fluidly in everyday speech.
Individual voice differences recognized, while underlying sound types allow comprehension in unfamiliar speech.