Category of informal fallacies in which the logical link (inference) between premises and conclusion is weak rather than entirely absent.
Differs from fallacies of relevance: premises do supply some evidence, but far less than required for a rational believer to accept the conclusion.
Typical pattern: premises contain a ‘shred’ of support that seems plausible at first glance, yet scrutiny shows it insufficient.
Practical significance: Recognizing weak-induction fallacies helps evaluate polls, news items, advertisements, political speeches, and courtroom arguments where incomplete evidence is routinely offered.
Ethical implication: Relying on such arguments may lead to unsound policies, wrongful convictions, or the spread of misinformation.
Appeal to Unqualified Authority (Argumentum ad Verecundiam)
Core Idea
An argument cites an authority or witness who lacks the appropriate credibility for the point at issue.
Common credibility failures
Lack of relevant expertise or credentials.
Bias, prejudice, or financial/political motive that undermines neutrality.
Intentional deceit or desire to spread misinformation.
Inability to perceive/remember accurately (e.g., poor eyesight, bad memory, intoxication).
Classical structure
Premise: Authority A states that proposition P is true.
Conclusion: Therefore, P is true.
Hidden (illicit) premise: Authority A is reliable on this topic.
Illustrative examples
Medical doctor pronounces on nuclear fusion: Dr. Bradshaw (expert in medicine, not physics) claims muonic atoms will create room-temperature fusion ➔ insufficient authority.
Tobacco executive testifies cigarettes are non-addictive: James W. Johnston (financially motivated, biased) ➔ trustworthiness compromised.
Nearly blind witness claims to have seen a stabbing from 100 yards at twilight ➔ perceptual competence absent, testimony unreliable.
Why fallacious?
Expertise is domain-specific; lack of domain overlap eliminates evidential weight.
Faulty perception/memory means empirical premises become doubtful.
Real-world relevance: Media often quote celebrities on scientific matters (vaccines, climate) producing persuasive but unsound public opinion.
Appeal to Ignorance (Argumentum ad Ignorantiam)
Core Idea
From the fact that something has not been proven true (or false), the arguer concludes it is false (or true).
Typical form
Premise: No one has proved proposition P (or \neg P).
Conclusion: Therefore, P is false (or true).
Key diagnostic features
Issue is inherently difficult or (currently) impossible to test.
The premises produce zero positive evidence regarding truth-value.
Cited investigators lack relevant expertise or remain unnamed.
Example (fallacious)
“Centuries of attempts to verify astrology have failed; hence astrology is nonsense.”
• Premise lacks positive evidence; the failure might be due to poor methods, not the idea’s falsity.
Legitimate non-fallacious variant
When qualified experts in a relevant field conduct thorough searches yet still fail to find evidence, non-existence becomes the most reasonable conclusion.
Example: Decades of scientific experiments failed to detect the luminiferous aether ➔ reasonable to infer non-existence.
Practical caveat: Science often moves from ‘absence of evidence’ to ‘evidence of absence’ only when detection methods are adequately sensitive and the search exhaustive.
Hasty Generalization (Converse Accident)
Core Idea
Drawing a broad conclusion about an entire group/population from an unrepresentative sample.
Two main sample defects
Sample size too small (insufficient n).
Sample biased (not randomly or objectively selected), even if numerically large.
Example 1: Small sample
“Money managers are all thieves; look at Bernie Madoff, Robert Stanford, Raj Rajaratnam.” ➔ Three notorious cases do not justify condemning every manager.
Example 2: Large but biased sample
Survey of 100{,}000 voters in conservative Orange County shows 68\% support for the Republican candidate; author concludes the Republican will win statewide. The county’s ideological skew makes the sample unrepresentative.
Statistical perspective
Representative sampling requires both adequate size (reduce margin of error \pm\varepsilon) and randomness (avoid systematic error). Ignoring these conditions invalidates inductive leap.
Ethical/political impact: Misleading polls may shape voter expectations, influence donations, or depress turnout.
False Cause (Non Causa Pro Causa family)
Overall theme
Argument mistakenly assumes a causal linkage that is nonexistent, reversed, or oversimplified.
Three main varieties
Post hoc ergo propter hoc (“after this, therefore because of this”)
• Confuses temporal succession with causation.
• Example: Cheerleaders wear blue ribbons ➔ team loses ➔ conclude ribbons cause losses.
Non causa pro causa (“not the cause for the cause” / causal reversal)
• Picks a factor that correlates with the effect but is actually an effect—or unrelated.
• Example: “Executives earn >100{,}000, so raising Ferguson’s pay to 100{,}000 will make him successful.” Salary likely an effect, not the cause, of executive success.
Oversimplified cause
• Complex phenomenon explained by citing only one among many causal factors.
• Example: Declining school quality blamed solely on teachers; ignores funding, class size, curriculum, socio-economic factors, etc.
Analytical tools
Causal inference requires controlling for confounding variables, temporal precedence, and plausibility. Methods: randomized controlled trials, statistical regressions.
Practical danger: Faulty causal attributions can drive ineffective policies (e.g., raising pay without training) or scapegoat groups unfairly (teachers).
Slippery Slope
Core Idea
Arguer claims that a seemingly innocuous first step will inevitably trigger a chain of events culminating in extreme (usually catastrophic) outcome.
Logical structure
If action A occurs, then event B will follow.
B leads to C, C to D … eventually disaster Z.
Therefore, avoid action A to prevent Z.
Fallacious when
Links in the causal chain are not substantiated; probability of transition between each step is low.
Example
Failure to outlaw pornography ➔ rise in sex crimes ➔ moral decay ➔ general crime wave ➔ collapse of civilization. Each link is speculative, unsupported.
Evaluation guideline: Require independent evidence for each causal connection; consider mechanisms, statistical data, possibility of intervention points.
Ethical concern: Slippery slope rhetoric can stifle reforms (e.g., same-sex marriage, drug decriminalization) by exaggerating remote risks.
Weak Analogy (Faulty Analogy)
Core Idea
Reasoning by comparing two entities that share some properties p,q,r, but inferring a further similarity z without sufficient justification.
Standard form
Entity A has features p,q,r,z.
Entity B has features p,q,r.
Therefore, B has feature z.
Diagnostic question
Are p,q,r causally or systematically connected to z? If not, analogy is weak.
Example
Comparing car breakdown and heart attack: A passing mechanic (like any driver) has no obligation to stop; therefore, a passing physician has no obligation to assist a heart-attack victim. Analogy fails because professional ethical duties of physicians (Hippocratic tradition, legal Good Samaritan statutes) link medical expertise to emergency aid, unlike mechanical skill.
Strengthening an analogy requires demonstrating relevant similarities (shared underlying principles) and disarming disanalogies.
Study & Practice Tips
When encountering an inductive argument, identify:
Type of inference (authority, sample, cause, analogy, ignorance).