Myers' Psychology for the AP Course (M19)
Module 19
Visual Organization and Interpretation
Perceptual Organization
German psychologists noticed that people who are given a cluster of sensations tend to organize them into a gestalt
Gestalt: an organized whole. Gestalt psychologists emphasized our tendency to integrate pieces of information into meaningful wholes.
Form Perception
Figure and Ground
Figure-ground
The organization of the visual field into objects (the figures) that stand out from their surroundings (the ground).
Grouping
The perceptual tendency to organize stimuli into coherent groups.
Our mind brings order and form to other stimuli by following certain rules for grouping
These rules illustrate how the perceived whole differs from the sum of its parts
Examples:
Proximity
We group nearby figures together
Continuity:
We perceive smooth, continuous patterns rather than discontinuous ones
Closure:
We fill in gaps to create complete, whole object
These principles help us construct reality, but sometimes they lead us astray
Depth Perception
The ability to see objects in three dimensions although the images that strike the retina are two-dimensional; allows us to judge distance.
From the two-dimensional images falling unto the retinas, it can organize three-dimensional perceptions that allow us to estimate distance
Eleanor Gibson
She wondered: Would a toddler peering over the rim perceive the dangerous drop-off and draw back?
To answer the question, Gibson and Richard Walk designed a series of experiments using a visual cliff
Visual cliff: a laboratory device for testing depth perception in infants and young animals.
They tried to convince infants to crawl out onto the glass—an area that looked like the edge of a cliff—but most infants refused to do so, which indicated that they could perceive depth
Depth perception is partly innate and partly learning
Binocular Cues
A depth cue, such as retinal disparity, that depends on the use of two eyes.
Binocular cues are used to judge the distance of nearby objects
Cues:
Convergence
The inward angle of the eyes focusing on a near object
Retinal disparity
A binocular cue for perceiving depth. By comparing retinal images from the two eyes, the brain computes distance—the greater the disparity (difference) between the two images, the closer the object.
Monocular Cues
A depth cue, such as interposition or linear perspective, available to either eye alone.
Helps us judge (for example) whether a person is 10 or 100 meters away
Motion Perception
The brain, normally, computes motion based partly on its assumption that shrinking objects are retreating and enlarging objects are approaching, but people are imperfect at motion perception
The brain is sometimes tricked into believing what it is not seeing
When large and small objects move at the same speed, the large objects appear to move more slowly
The brain perceives a rapid series of slightly varying images as continuous movement
We construct that motion in our heads
Phi phenomenon
An illusion of movement created when two or more adjacent lights blink on and off in quick succession.
Visual Organization and Interpretation
Relative height
We perceive objects higher in our field of vision as farther away
Relative size
If we assume two objects are similar in size, most people perceive the one that cast the smaller retinal images as farther away
Interposition
If one object partially blocks our view of another, we perceive it as closer
Relative motion
As we move, objects that are actually stable may appear to move
Linear perspective
Parallel lines appear to meet in the distance
Light and shadow
Shading produces a sense of depth consistent with our assumption that light comes from above
Perceptual Constancy
Perceiving objects as unchanging (having consistent color, brightness, shape, and size) even as illumination and retinal images change.
Regardless of the viewing angle, distance, and illumination, we can identify people and things in less time than it takes to draw a breath
Color and Brightness Constancies
Our experience of color depends on an object’s context
Color constancy
Perceiving familiar objects as having consistent color, even if changing illumination alters the wavelengths reflected by the object.
Because we construct our perceptions, we can simultaneously accept alternative objective and subjective realities
Brightness constancy similarly depends on context
We perceive an object as having a constant brightness even as its illumination varies
This perception of constancy depends on relative luminance
The amount of light an object reflects relative to its surroundings
Context governs our perceptions
Shape and Size Constancies
Sometimes an object whose actual shape cannot change seems to change shape with the angle of our view
Size constancy helps us perceive an object as having an unchanging size, even while our distance from it varies
Even in size-distance judgements, the object’s context is always considered
The interplay between perceived size and perceived distance helps explain several well-known illusions
Perpetual organizations applies to other senses as well
Listening to our own language, we can automatically hear distinct words
We can organize a string of letters into words that make an intelligible phrase
THEDOGATEMEAT is more likely seen as “the dog ate meat”, rather than “the do gate me at”
Perceptual Interpretation
Immanuel Kant
German philosopher
Maintained that knowledge comes from our inborn ways of organizing sensory experiences
John Locke
British philosopher
Argued that through our experiences we also learn to perceive the world
Experience and Visual Perception
Restored Vision and Sensory Restriction
William Molyneux
Wondered whether “a man born blind, and now adult, taught by his touch to distinguish between a cube and a sphere” could visually distinguish the two
John Locke answered nom as the man would never have learned to see the difference
Was put to test with adults who were born blind but later gained sight, but remained unable to visually recognize objects
Perceptual Adaptation
The ability to adjust to changed sensory input, including an artificially displaced or even inverted visual field.
Humans can adapt to distorting lenses quickly
Even when given a lens that turns the world upside down
Experience guides, sustains, and maintains the brain pathways that enable our perceptions
Smaller text = Bolded vocab in the textbook
Bolded = Headings
Module 19
Visual Organization and Interpretation
Perceptual Organization
German psychologists noticed that people who are given a cluster of sensations tend to organize them into a gestalt
Gestalt: an organized whole. Gestalt psychologists emphasized our tendency to integrate pieces of information into meaningful wholes.
Form Perception
Figure and Ground
Figure-ground
The organization of the visual field into objects (the figures) that stand out from their surroundings (the ground).
Grouping
The perceptual tendency to organize stimuli into coherent groups.
Our mind brings order and form to other stimuli by following certain rules for grouping
These rules illustrate how the perceived whole differs from the sum of its parts
Examples:
Proximity
We group nearby figures together
Continuity:
We perceive smooth, continuous patterns rather than discontinuous ones
Closure:
We fill in gaps to create complete, whole object
These principles help us construct reality, but sometimes they lead us astray
Depth Perception
The ability to see objects in three dimensions although the images that strike the retina are two-dimensional; allows us to judge distance.
From the two-dimensional images falling unto the retinas, it can organize three-dimensional perceptions that allow us to estimate distance
Eleanor Gibson
She wondered: Would a toddler peering over the rim perceive the dangerous drop-off and draw back?
To answer the question, Gibson and Richard Walk designed a series of experiments using a visual cliff
Visual cliff: a laboratory device for testing depth perception in infants and young animals.
They tried to convince infants to crawl out onto the glass—an area that looked like the edge of a cliff—but most infants refused to do so, which indicated that they could perceive depth
Depth perception is partly innate and partly learning
Binocular Cues
A depth cue, such as retinal disparity, that depends on the use of two eyes.
Binocular cues are used to judge the distance of nearby objects
Cues:
Convergence
The inward angle of the eyes focusing on a near object
Retinal disparity
A binocular cue for perceiving depth. By comparing retinal images from the two eyes, the brain computes distance—the greater the disparity (difference) between the two images, the closer the object.
Monocular Cues
A depth cue, such as interposition or linear perspective, available to either eye alone.
Helps us judge (for example) whether a person is 10 or 100 meters away
Motion Perception
The brain, normally, computes motion based partly on its assumption that shrinking objects are retreating and enlarging objects are approaching, but people are imperfect at motion perception
The brain is sometimes tricked into believing what it is not seeing
When large and small objects move at the same speed, the large objects appear to move more slowly
The brain perceives a rapid series of slightly varying images as continuous movement
We construct that motion in our heads
Phi phenomenon
An illusion of movement created when two or more adjacent lights blink on and off in quick succession.
Visual Organization and Interpretation
Relative height
We perceive objects higher in our field of vision as farther away
Relative size
If we assume two objects are similar in size, most people perceive the one that cast the smaller retinal images as farther away
Interposition
If one object partially blocks our view of another, we perceive it as closer
Relative motion
As we move, objects that are actually stable may appear to move
Linear perspective
Parallel lines appear to meet in the distance
Light and shadow
Shading produces a sense of depth consistent with our assumption that light comes from above
Perceptual Constancy
Perceiving objects as unchanging (having consistent color, brightness, shape, and size) even as illumination and retinal images change.
Regardless of the viewing angle, distance, and illumination, we can identify people and things in less time than it takes to draw a breath
Color and Brightness Constancies
Our experience of color depends on an object’s context
Color constancy
Perceiving familiar objects as having consistent color, even if changing illumination alters the wavelengths reflected by the object.
Because we construct our perceptions, we can simultaneously accept alternative objective and subjective realities
Brightness constancy similarly depends on context
We perceive an object as having a constant brightness even as its illumination varies
This perception of constancy depends on relative luminance
The amount of light an object reflects relative to its surroundings
Context governs our perceptions
Shape and Size Constancies
Sometimes an object whose actual shape cannot change seems to change shape with the angle of our view
Size constancy helps us perceive an object as having an unchanging size, even while our distance from it varies
Even in size-distance judgements, the object’s context is always considered
The interplay between perceived size and perceived distance helps explain several well-known illusions
Perpetual organizations applies to other senses as well
Listening to our own language, we can automatically hear distinct words
We can organize a string of letters into words that make an intelligible phrase
THEDOGATEMEAT is more likely seen as “the dog ate meat”, rather than “the do gate me at”
Perceptual Interpretation
Immanuel Kant
German philosopher
Maintained that knowledge comes from our inborn ways of organizing sensory experiences
John Locke
British philosopher
Argued that through our experiences we also learn to perceive the world
Experience and Visual Perception
Restored Vision and Sensory Restriction
William Molyneux
Wondered whether “a man born blind, and now adult, taught by his touch to distinguish between a cube and a sphere” could visually distinguish the two
John Locke answered nom as the man would never have learned to see the difference
Was put to test with adults who were born blind but later gained sight, but remained unable to visually recognize objects
Perceptual Adaptation
The ability to adjust to changed sensory input, including an artificially displaced or even inverted visual field.
Humans can adapt to distorting lenses quickly
Even when given a lens that turns the world upside down
Experience guides, sustains, and maintains the brain pathways that enable our perceptions
Smaller text = Bolded vocab in the textbook
Bolded = Headings