Infant Language Acquisition and Bilingualism – Study Notes
Infant Language Acquisition and Bilingualism – innate language abilities in infants
Infants are primed to learn language from birth; language is often described as an innate ability, comparable to other basic skills like grasping or sucking.
Babies from bilingual households can grow up fluent in two languages; language learning doesn’t require special training—just exposure.
Classic idea: language is something we’re born knowing how to do; this innate capacity supports learning any language given the right input.
Early sound discrimination and perceptual narrowing
In the first months, babies can distinguish very similar sounds, even from foreign languages.
Experimental setup (common in this field): play a sound from a foreign language on loop for a baby around 4 ext{ months} old. When the baby’s interest wanes, switch to a new, similar sound.
Indicator of noticing a difference: baby’s behavior changes, e.g., looking around, sucking more or harder on a pacifier.
By about 12 months (one year old), many babies stop noticing certain non-native sound differences as their brains prune unused connections and focus on their native language sounds.
Mechanism: neural connections related to non-native sounds are weakened while those for native-language sounds are strengthened (neural pruning/neuroplasticity).
Conclusion: early exposure shapes perceptual emphasis; by around the first year, infants’ brains increasingly tune in to the language they’ve heard most.
Important takeaway: bilingual exposure requires equal exposure to both languages to develop bilingual processing; no special training is necessary.
Myths about bilingual development
Common claim: bilingual kids develop more slowly or have speech delays. Research shows this is not generally true.
The same holds for babies who grow up with one spoken language and one signed language (e.g., English and American Sign Language): they can keep their languages separate thanks to perceptual skills.
By about three years old, children who are exposed to two languages can use perceptual cues to decide which language to speak in different contexts (e.g., speaking English with friends, Cantonese with mom).
When children deliberately switch languages (code-switching), this is a normal skill reflecting language regulation in social contexts.
Code-switching and language regulation
Definition: Code-switching is the intentional switching from one language to another in different social situations.
By around age 3, children can regulate which language to use with different people or settings.
EEG studies of infant language processing (monolingual vs bilingual brains)
Source: University of Washington EEG study comparing monolingual and bilingual infants.
At 6 ext{ months}: infants are exposed to recordings of English and Spanish.
Monolingual babies show a spike on the EEG whenever a mismatched sound occurs (e.g., a Spanish sound among English sounds, or vice versa), indicating they notice a difference.
Bilingual babies do not show this spike, indicating a different processing pattern at this age.
By 10 - 12 months: developmental shift occurs.
Monolingual babies’ brains respond when a sound in their native language interrupts a string of foreign sounds (but not the reverse).
Bilingual babies shift from not noticing differences to noticing both kinds of mismatches (they become sensitive to both language contexts).
Interpretation: Monolingual brains tend to solidify connections faster to prepare for a single primary language, while bilingual brains stay more flexible and continue adapting to multiple languages at a later stage.
Developmental advantages observed in bilingual infants
A study in the journal Science found that bilingual babies may be better at learning rules and switching between them than monolingual babies.
Example from the study: 1-year-old infants were taught to look at a specific location on a screen when hearing a certain pattern of sounds to reveal a toy; when the pattern changed and the toy location moved, bilingual infants were better at figuring out the new rule and looking in the correct places.
This suggests bilingualism may confer advantages in rule learning and cognitive flexibility early in development.
Cognitive and real-world benefits in adulthood
Across studies, bilingual adults often show:
Better focus and greater ability to switch between tasks (cognitive flexibility).
Slower cognitive decline with aging compared to monolinguals.
Important nuance: these benefits reflect one possible developmental pathway; the brain remains highly plastic, and non-bilingual individuals can still achieve strong cognitive outcomes.
Takeaway: bilingual experience is one route to certain cognitive advantages, but it is not the sole predictor of intelligence or cognitive health.
Connections to broader concepts and real-world relevance
Neuroplasticity: early language exposure shapes neural wiring; pruning and strengthening of connections reflect experience-driven brain development.
Perceptual narrowing: young brains focus on the sounds and patterns of the language environment they are exposed to, filtering out others over time.
Perceptual and cognitive flexibility: exposure to multiple languages may foster flexible rule learning and switching capabilities.
Educational and social implications: supporting early, equal exposure to multiple languages can help children develop bilingual skills without sacrificing development in either language; code-switching is a natural part of bilingual communication.
Key terms and concepts to remember
Innate language ability
Bilingual vs monolingual development
Equal exposure to two languages
Perceptual narrowing and neural pruning
Code-switching
EEG (electroencephalography) and neural mismatches
Neuroplasticity and flexible brain wiring
Rule learning and cognitive switching
Long-term cognitive benefits and aging effects
Numerical references and study details
Ages and time points:
Initial sound exposure test around 4 months
Follow-up observations around 12 months (one year) to assess changes
Early perception changes observed at approximately 6 months
Later development noted around 10 - 12 months
Language switching observed by approximately 3 years old
Experimental stimuli examples include: English vs. Spanish sound streams
Practical takeaways for learners and educators
Exposure matters: ensure infants and children have ample, balanced exposure to both languages to support bilingual development.
Code-switching is a natural, healthy part of bilingual communication and should be viewed as a normal skill rather than a problem.
Bilingualism is associated with cognitive advantages that can persist into adulthood, but a lack of bilingual exposure does not doom cognitive development—brain plasticity supports multiple developmental pathways.
When teaching or learning languages, focusing on meaningful social and communicative contexts can help children correctly assign languages to contexts and speakers.