Learning | The process of acquiring, through experience, new and relatively enduring information or behaviors |
Associative learning | Learning that certain events occur together. The events may be two stimuli (as in classical conditioning) or as a response and its consequences (as in operant conditioning). |
Cognitive learning | The acquisition of mental information, whether by observing events, by watching others, or through language |
Behaviorism (Behavioral perspective) | The view that psychology (1) should be an objective science that (2) studies behavior without reference to mental processes. Most research psychologists today agree with (1) but not with (2). |
Stimulus | Any event or situation that evokes a response |
Classical conditioning | A type of learning in which we link two or more stimuli; as a result, to illustrate with Pavlov’s famous experiment, the first stimulus (a tone) comes to elicit behavior (drooling) in anticipation of the second stimulus (food). |
Neutral stimulus (NS) | In classical conditioning, a stimulus that elicits no response before conditioning |
Unconditioned stimulus (UCS) | In classical conditioning, a stimulus that unconditionally — naturally and automatically — triggers an unconditioned response |
Unconditioned response (UCR) | In classical conditioning, an unlearned, naturally occurring response (such as salivation) to an unconditioned stimulus (such as when food is in the mouth) |
Conditioned stimulus (CS) | In classical conditioning, an originally neutral stimulus that, after association with an unconditioned stimulus, comes to trigger a conditioned response |
Conditioned response (CR) | In classical conditioning, a learned response to a previously neutral (but now conditioned) stimulus |
Higher-order conditioning | A procedure in which the conditioned stimulus in one conditioning experience is paired with a new neutral stimulus, creating a second (often weaker) stimulus. For example, an animal that has learned that a tone predicts food might then learn that a light predicts the tone and begin responding to the light alone. |
Acquisition | In classical conditioning, the initial stage, when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned response. (In operant conditioning, the strengthening of a reinforced response.) |
Extinction | In classical conditioning, the diminishing of a conditioned response when an unconditioned stimulus does not follow a conditioned stimulus. (In operant conditioning, when a response is no longer reinforced.) |
Spontaneous recovery | The reappearance, after a pause, of a weakened conditioned response |
Stimulus generalization | In classical conditioning, the tendency, once a response has been conditioned, for stimuli similar to the conditioned stimulus to elicit similar responses. |
Stimulus discrimination | In classical conditioning, the learned ability to distinguish between a conditioned stimulus and other stimuli that have not been associated with a conditioned stimulus. |
Counterconditioning | Counseling technique used to help people deal with fears, where subjects are presented with a feared stimulus, but in a relaxed environment, until extinction occurs |
One-trial conditioning | A type of learning where a single pairing of a conditioned stimulus with an unconditioned stimulus is enough to create a conditioned response, meaning an organism learns a new behavior after just one exposure. Often seen in situations with strong survival implications like taste aversion. |
Biological preparedness | A predisposition to learn associations, such as between taste and nausea, that have survival value |
Habituation | Decreasing responsiveness with repeated stimulation |
Operant conditioning | A type of learning in which behavior becomes more likely to occur if followed by a reinforcer or less likely to occur if followed by a punisher |
The law of effect | Thorndike’s principle that behaviors followed by favorable (or reinforcing) consequences become more likely, and that behaviors followed by unfavorable (or punishing) consequences become less likely |
Operant chamber | In operant conditioning research, an enclosed device (also known as a Skinner box) containing a bar or key that an animal can manipulate to obtain a food or water reinforcer; attached devices record the animals rate of bar pressing or key pecking |
Reinforcement | In operant conditioning, any event that strengthens the behavior it follows |
Positive reinforcement | Increasing behaviors by presenting positive stimuli, such as food. A positive reinforcer is any stimulus that, when presented after a response, strengthens the response. |
Negative reinforcement | Increasing behaviors by stopping or reducing negative stimuli, such as shock. A negative reinforcer is any stimulus that, when removed after a response, strengthens the response. (Note: negative reinforcement is not punishment.) |
Primary reinforcers | An innately reinforcing stimulus, such as one that satisfies a biological need |
Secondary reinforcers | A stimulus that gains its reinforcing power through its association with a primary reinforcer; also known as a conditioned reinforcer |
Reinforcement generalization | In operant conditioning, when responses learned in one situation are applied in other, similar situations |
Reinforcement discrimination | The ability to distinguish responses that are reinforced from similar responses that are not reinforced |
Punishment | Any event that tends to decrease the behavior that it follows. |
Positive punishment | An event that administers an aversive, or unpleasant, stimulus as a way of decreasing the behavior that it follows |
Negative punishment | The withdrawal of a rewarding stimulus, as a way of decreasing the behavior that it follows |
Shaping | An operant conditioning procedure in which reinforcers guide behavior toward closer and closer approximations of the desired behavior |
Instinctive drift | The tendency of learned behavior to gradually revert to biologically predisposed patterns |
Continuous reinforcement | Reinforcing the desired response every time it occurs |
Partial reinforcement | Reinforcing a response only part of the time; results in slower acquisition of a response but much greater resistance to extinction than does continuous reinforcement |
Fixed-interval schedule | In operant conditioning, a reinforcement schedule that reinforces a response only after a specified time has elapsed |
Variable-interval schedule | In operant conditioning, a reinforcement schedule that reinforces a response at unpredictable time intervals |
Fixed-ratio schedule | In operant conditioning, a reinforcement schedule that reinforces a response only after specified number of responses |
Variable-ratio schedule | In operant conditioning, a reinforcement schedule that reinforces a response after an unpredictable number of responses |
Social learning theory | The perspective in psychology that focuses on how we learn by observing and imitating others |
Modeling | The process of observing and imitating a specific behavior |
Vicarious conditioning | Learning that occurs by watching how others’ behaviors are reinforced or punished |
Insight learning | Solving problems through sudden cognition, without any association, consequence, or model being presented; contrasts with strategy-based solutions |
Latent learning | Learning that occurs but is not apparent until there is an incentive to demonstrate it |
Cognitive map | A mental representation of the layout of one’s environment |