Psychology of Human Relationships
Definition of Culture
Culture encompasses attitudes, behaviors, and symbols shared by a large group and passed down through generations.
Shiraev and Levy (2004) define culture as a set of beliefs, values, superstitions, stereotypes, norms, customs, traditions, and fashions.
Sociocultural Events Influence Relationships:
Sociocultural events shape individuals' preferences in forming relationships.
Cultural dynamics impact who individuals are inclined to build connections with.
Components of Culture:
Attitudes:
Beliefs, encompassing political, religious, and moral perspectives.
Values that guide individuals' priorities and principles.
Superstitions and stereotypes influence perceptions.
Behaviors:
Norms dictate acceptable actions within a society.
Customs and traditions shape social interactions.
Fashions influence lifestyle choices and preferences.
Impact on Perception of Romantic Love:
Cultural norms and values influence how romantic love is perceived.
Defines societal expectations regarding the intensity and expression of romantic feelings.
Criteria for Attraction:
Cultural norms dictate what qualities are deemed attractive in a potential partner.
Influences preferences related to physical appearance, social status, and educational background.
Guidance on Relationship Initiation:
Cultural norms guide when individuals are expected to initiate romantic relationships.
Societal expectations regarding the appropriate timing for entering into romantic relations.
Role in Partner Selection:
Cultural values impact the characteristics considered desirable in a partner.
Shapes individuals' criteria for selecting a mate, including factors like personality, values, and social status.
Arranged Marriages and Cultural Influence:
Some cultures practice arranged marriages, emphasizing the role of societal expectations in relationship formation.
Reflects the influence of cultural norms on familial involvement in selecting partners.
Gender Roles and Relationship Dynamics:
Cultural expectations regarding gender roles impact power dynamics and division of labor within relationships.
Societal attitudes toward evolving gender roles can influence the nature of relationships.
Cultural Diversity in Relationships:
Sociocultural factors are crucial in intercultural relationships.
Managing cultural differences becomes a central aspect of relationship formation.
Cross-Cultural Differences in Attractiveness:
Cunningham et al. (1995) observed cultural variations in the perception of attractive bodies.
Example: African American men found larger women more attractive than white men, highlighting cultural specificity in beauty standards.
Cross-Cultural Variances in Female Physical Attributes:
Swami et al. (2006) compared UK and Japanese perceptions of an attractive female body.
Findings: Japanese preferences differed significantly, emphasizing lower body mass indexes (BMI) and specific body shapes compared to Britons.
BMI as a Universal Determinant:
Swami and Tovee (2005) found BMI as the primary determinant of female physical attractiveness in both the UK and Malaysia.
Cultural Variation: Attractiveness preferences varied along a socioeconomic gradient, with urban participants favoring lower BMIs compared to rural counterparts.
Buss's Cross-Cultural Study on Relationship Formation:
Buss (1994) investigated factors in relationship formation across 37 cultures.
Universals: Men prefer younger mates, and women prefer older mates.
Cultural Differences: Varied importance placed on characteristics; e.g., virginity is highly valued in China, Taiwan, India, and Iran, but less so in Finland, Denmark, Sweden, France, and Germany.
Impact of Rural vs. Urban Contexts:
The importance of love in relationship formation may vary with the level of urbanization and survival pressures.
Lindholm's suggestion: In more rural communities where survival is a primary concern, love may be less important compared to other factors.
Globalization and Changing Cultural Constructs:
The spread of Western approaches to relationships worldwide is noted, possibly influenced by globalization.
Romantic love, while not universal, is becoming more widespread as cultures adopt aspects of Western relationship ideals.
Passionate Love as Culturally Specific:
Goodwin (1995) argues that the notion of passionate love as a primary reason for forming serious relationships is culturally specific, particularly to Western cultures.
In more traditional societies, the emphasis is on loving the person one marries rather than marrying for passionate love.
Arranged Marriages as Sociocultural Norms:
In several cultures, arranged marriages are common, challenging the Western emphasis on passionate love as a basis for marriage.
Parents often play a central role in the decision-making process, sometimes presenting a range of potential partners for their child.
Lack of Individual Choice in Arranged Marriages:
In some cases of arranged marriages, the individuals involved may have little or no say in choosing their partners or deciding on the marriage.
The decision-making power often lies with parents or other family members.
Factors Contributing to Arranged Marriage Success:
In arranged marriages, the involvement of a third party (e.g., parents) is considered a factor contributing to their success.
Intellectual elements, along with emotional factors, play a role in decision-making.
Definition of Social Cues:
Social cues are visual or non-verbal characteristics in an environment that influence people's thoughts, emotions, and behaviors.
Examples of Social Cues:
Body Language:
Gestures, postures, and movements that convey information without verbal communication.
Example: A person crossing their arms may indicate defensiveness or resistance.
Facial Expressions:
Emotional expressions are conveyed through changes in facial muscles.
Example: A smile indicates happiness or approval, while a frown may convey disapproval or sadness.
Assumptions:
Biological Influence on Relationship Formation:
Assumes that biological events significantly influence individuals' preferences in forming relationships.
Survival Purpose of Heterosexual Relationships:
Explains that heterosexual relationships serve a survival purpose by facilitating the transmission of the best possible genetic combinations to offspring.
Evolutionary Arguments for Heterosexuality:
Attraction as an Adaptive Function:
Attraction is viewed as a product of evolution, serving as an adaptive function for the survival and reproduction of the species.
Gender-Specific Preferences:
Males:
Generally prefer younger women.
Pay attention to physical details like teeth, lip color, hair length and shine, hip size, and skin smoothness—characteristics representing the ability to produce offspring.
Females:
Generally prefer older men.
Pay attention to characteristics like ambition, wealth, intelligence, social status, energy levels, and good health—traits associated with the ability to provide for offspring.
Genetic Point of View:
Attraction, according to this perspective, has evolved as a genetically encoded trait.
Similarities and Differences:
Similarities:
Both genders pay attention to the appearance of a potential mate.
Both sexes value traits like 'kindness' and 'intelligence.'
Differences:
Males and females exhibit gender-specific preferences based on evolutionary needs for reproductive success.
Empirical Support:
The gender differences in attraction are supported by large-scale cross-cultural surveys, such as those conducted by Buss (1994, 1995).
Assumption:
The attraction between individuals is influenced by the combination of their genes, to produce healthy offspring.
Evidence for Assumption:
Attraction to pheromones carried in sweat, containing information about a person's immune system.
Assumption: Genetically different immune systems complement each other, potentially resulting in offspring with a robust immune system.
Role of MHC Genes:
Control of Immunological Self on-Self Discrimination:
MHC genes control immunological self-self discrimination, tissue rejection, and immune recognition of infectious diseases.
Importance for Health:
MHC genes play a crucial role in ensuring individuals stay healthy by regulating immune responses.
Sexual Selection and MHC Genes:
Avoiding Inbreeding:
MHC genes are assumed to be the product of sexual selection, aiming to improve the immune system of offspring and prevent inbreeding.
Studies on Mice:
Studies in house mice suggest that both males and females prefer mates with MHC-dissimilar genes, recognized through odor cues.
Studies in Humans:
Human studies, including those by Wedekind and Penn (2000), found associations between MHC-associated odors and mating preferences.
Cognitive Health Connection:
Facial symmetry is linked to greater cognitive health, with symmetry indicating the reliability and precision of developmental processes (Penke et al., 2009).
Supports the bodily integrity hypothesis, suggesting a correlation between scoring well on cognitive ability tests and a general tendency for overall body systems to be efficient and healthy (Deary, 2012).
Associations with Physical Health:
Facial symmetry is not only an indicator of cognitive health but is also associated with better physical health.
The bodily cues provided by facial symmetry may offer insights into both cognitive and physical well-being.
The Golden Ratio:
Facial symmetry has been suggested to be linked with the mathematical concept of the 'golden ratio.'
The golden ratio refers to pleasing and harmonious proportions that adhere to a specific mathematical equation.
The complexity of Achieving Symmetry:
Achieving facial symmetry is a complex task during human growth, involving billions of cell reproductions while maintaining a parallel structure.
Facial symmetry is considered a visible signal of genetic health, as it reflects successful and precise developmental processes.
Biological Origins and Cross-Cultural Consistency:
Perceiving facial symmetry has a clear cognitive element and is cross-cultural, suggesting biological origins.
The universality of appreciating facial symmetry across cultures indicates a shared, biologically rooted perception.
Limitations:
While facial symmetry is considered an indicator of attractiveness, there are cross-cultural differences in beauty standards.
Cultural factors can influence what is perceived as beautiful, emphasizing the interplay between biological and cultural influences.
Impact of Ovulation on Women's Attractiveness:
Ovulation is believed to influence women's preferences in men and their physical appearance, impacting their attractiveness to prospective partners.
Concealed Ovulation in Humans:
Unlike some other mammals, human females conceal ovulation, giving them more control over reproduction.
Concealing ovulation allows females to have more control and potentially cheat on mates without being detected during ovulation.
Definition:
The false correlation of positive characteristics, assuming that someone with a few positive traits possesses many positive characteristics.
Association with Physical Attractiveness:
The halo effect is sometimes linked to physical attractiveness, where people may assume that physically attractive individuals possess other positive traits.
Research Context:
Ahmad and Reid (2008) explored the connection between traditional roles, communication styles, and marital satisfaction.
Focused on South Asian couples in Canada, particularly those with traditional beliefs in marriage.
Methodology:
Married individuals of South Asian descent participated.
Only one partner per couple completed the questionnaire.
Questionnaire Content:
Explored attitudes toward marriage.
Examined communication styles within the marriage.
Assessed marital satisfaction.
Key Findings:
Traditional role beliefs did not necessarily lead to lower satisfaction.
The critical factor was the listening style in the relationship.
Listening Style and Satisfaction:
Couples with traditional beliefs but effective listening were more likely to be satisfied.
Emphasizes the importance of actively listening to understand a partner's feelings.
Insights from Arranged Marriages:
Arranged marriages with traditional beliefs can be satisfying.
Listening plays a pivotal role in producing satisfaction despite having fewer choices in partners.
Implications:
Communication, specifically active listening, may play a more significant role in relationship satisfaction than cultural factors influencing marriage.
Explanation of Behavior: Attribution involves explaining the behavior of others in our environment.
Emotional Influence: Attributions can be influenced by our emotional connection and feelings toward the person.
Social Exchange Theory (SET):
Basic Assumption: Relationships are maintained through continuous cost-benefit analysis.
Balance Requirement: Balance between partners' investments and returns is crucial for relationship stability.
Dependence on Perception: SET involves the perception of imbalances and inequality, which is subjective to individual viewpoints.
Student A study Carlson et al. (2014):
Focus: Analyzed data on housework and sexual frequency from the 2006 Marital and Relationship Survey.
Participants: 600 married and cohabiting low to moderate-income couples with children.
Findings:
Equal Housework and Sexual Frequency:
Couples sharing routine housework equally reported the highest sexual frequency (7.74 times a month).
Higher satisfaction and quality of the sexual relationship in these couples.
Gender Equality and Intimacy:
Changing Gender Roles: Men participating in housework and childcare are seen as fair.
Increased Intimacy: Perception of fairness in cost-benefit analysis leads to increased intimacy in Western societies.
Role of Expectations:
Traditional vs. Non-Traditional Expectations:
Traditional marriages may have different expectations regarding roles and contributions.
Clear roles in traditional settings may provide a framework for perceived fairness.
Norway Divorce Rates:
Traditional Roles vs. Gender Equality:
Despite Norway's gender equality, couples with more traditional roles had lower divorce rates.
Suggests that clarity in roles and expectations aligns with perceived fairness.
Sociocultural changes, particularly in the West, have led to increased empowerment and financial independence for women.
Shifts in cultural norms and economic structures have influenced women's roles in the workforce and relationships.
Statistical Trends (Office for National Statistics, 2013):
The employment rate of women aged 16 to 64 rose from 53% in 1971 to 66% in 2012.
Concurrently, there has been a decline in employment for men.
Factors Influencing Sociocultural Change:
Rise of the Service Sector:
Shift from heavy industry to the service sector, favoring opportunities for women.
Women actively developing careers in diverse fields.
Impact on Relationships:
Career development requires time and energy, potentially delaying serious or long-term relationships.
Pressure on women due to biological time constraints related to fertility.
Men, not facing the same fertility decline, may feel less urgency to commit in the early stages of a relationship.
Relationship Dynamics: Early vs. Later Stages:
Early Stages:
Women may feel pressure to manage both careers and the desire to have children.
Men may perceive less urgency regarding commitment due to a lack of similar biological time constraints.
Later Stages:
Increased financial independence for women.
Accumulation of financial capital, such as shared property ownership.
Women are more capable of supporting themselves outside marriage.
Impact on Freedom of Choice:
Women have more freedom of choice in later relationship stages:
Financial independence.
Easier and cheaper divorce options.
Less Stigma for Divorce in Western Cultures
Introduction of No-Fault Divorce:
Initiated in some US states in the 1950s.
Couples were no longer required to prove fault; citing a broken-down marriage was sufficient.
By 1970, most US states adopted laws allowing no-fault divorces.
Impact on Divorce Rates:
Divorce Rates in the US:
1940-1965: The divorce rate remained near ten divorces for every 1000 married women.
1979: The rate increased to 20 divorces for every 1000 married women.
Factors Contributing to Increased Divorce Rates:
Adoption of no-fault divorce laws.
Changing societal attitudes toward divorce.
Changing Societal Influences
Decline of Religious Influence:
Religion's diminished role in influencing relationship decisions.
Couples are more likely to seek divorce when facing challenges.
Legal Relaxations:
Legal changes making divorce more accessible.
Impact on Stigma
Reduced Stigma:
No-fault divorce reduces the stigma associated with divorce.
Couples can choose divorce without attributing blame to one party.
Attitude Shifts:
Changing societal attitudes towards divorce.
Divorce becomes a more acceptable option for couples facing challenges.
Increased Life Expectancy
Health Improvements:
Better healthcare and education contribute to longer, healthier lives.
The overall increase in life expectancy in Western developed economies.
Changing Perspectives:
People no longer view late middle age as the end of life.
A shift towards starting second major relationships in extended life cycles.
Impact on Sexual Health
STD Rates:
As couples end long-term relationships, STD rates among older individuals increase.
In the USA (2013), people aged 55 and older accounted for 26% of those living with HIV.
Medication and Sexual Activity:
Introduction of medications for erectile dysfunction for older men.
Enables older individuals to engage in sexual activity in later years.
Lack of Education:
Older men tend to be less educated about STD risks.
May not be well-informed about safe practices.
Perceived Low Risk:
Older individuals may perceive themselves at low risk for STDs.
Contributes to the spread of STDs in this age group.
Inclusive Fitness
Definition:
The ability of an individual organism to pass on its genes to the next generation.
Consider direct descendants and shared genes of close relatives.
Explanation:
Enhances evolutionary success by promoting the reproduction and survival of genetically similar individuals.
Underpins kin selection theory, emphasizing intragroup cooperation as an adaptation.
Intragroup Cooperation and Kin Selection
Behavioral Adaptations:
Individuals engage in behaviors benefiting their ingroup, even at personal cost.
Motivation to help genetically similar individuals within the group.
Kin selection theory suggests intragroup cooperation as an evolutionary adaptation.
Intergroup Competitiveness
Alternative Adaptation:
Intergroup competitiveness is considered an adaptation.
Non-human primates exhibit sensitivity to social hierarchies and group distinctions.
Competitiveness is observed in more primitive species, responding to situational factors.
Examples from Non-Human Primates:
Primate Behavior:
Non-human primates show sensitivity to individual rankings within groups.
Recognition of social category fault lines, distinguishing between ingroup and outgroups.
Primitive Species and Competitiveness:
Sensitivity to Situational Factors:
Primitive species, such as beetle larvae and cockroaches, exhibit competitiveness.
Sensitivity to situational factors, including the number of potential competitors.
Conceptual Framework
Developed by Tajfel and Turner (1979).
Assumes natural division of social environment into ingroups (us) and outgroups (them or the other).
Social Categorization:
Reduction of Differences:
Reduces perceived differences within the ingroup.
Reduces perceived differences in the outgroup.
Increases perceived differences between the ingroup and outgroup.
Category Accentuation Effect:
Exaggeration of Differences:
Exaggeration of ingroup and outgroup differences.
Accentuation of intragroup similarities.
Social Identity Process:
Assimilation:
Individuals assimilate into their group by adopting the group's identity.
Behave in ways consistent with group behavior.
The group becomes the person's ingroup.
Social Comparison:
Identification and Comparison:
Individuals categorize themselves within a group.
Compare their ingroup with another group (outgroup).
Positive distinctiveness for self-esteem enhancement.
Ethnocentrism:
Ingroup Bias:
Positive behaviors of ingroup attributed to dispositional factors.
Negative behaviors of ingroup attributed to situational factors.
Positive behaviors of outgroup attributed to situational factors.
Negative behaviors of outgroup attributed to dispositional factors.
Stereotyping:
Fixed Images:
Fixed and oversimplified images of individuals or groups.
Occurs when individuals are perceived as members of an outgroup.
Assumes shared characteristics among outgroup members.
Perceptions of Group Homogeneity:
Linked to Social Identity:
Stereotypes are influenced by social identity processes.
Group homogeneity perceptions apply to both ingroups and outgroups.
Group Dynamics:
Definition:
System of behaviors and psychological processes within social ingroups.
Study applicable to understanding prosocial behavior, prejudice, discrimination, and violence.
Intragroup and Intergroup Dynamics:
Ingroups and outgroups are defined through comparisons and contrasts.
Cooperation:
Definition: A prosocial process where individuals work together for mutual benefit.
Contrast with Competition:
Competition: Individuals work toward selfish, non-shareable goals.
Cooperation usually occurs within ingroups as an intragroup process.
Enhancement of Organized Groups:
Shared group identity increases cooperative behavior (Brewer, 2008).
The viability of organized groups is boosted when members act to enhance group functioning (Tyler & Blader, 2000).
Tyler and Blader's Types of Cooperation Within Ingroups:
Following Group Rules:
Conducting Work on Behalf of the Group:
Intention to Stay Within the Group:
Intention has a cognitive nature but also involves social signaling.
A lack of clear intention may be interpreted as a lack of motivation to cooperate.
Two Types of Basic Cooperative Behavior:
Mandatory (Required) Behaviors:
Specified for particular roles in a group.
Originates from the group or group leader.
Discretionary (Non-Required) Behaviors:
Goes beyond required actions.
Originates from a group member, not the group.
SIT's Role in Ingroup Cooperation:
Individuals cooperate with their group as long as it provides a positive social identity.
If not, individuals might be tempted to change group membership (Doosje et al., 1999).
Ongoing cognitive process: Constantly measuring perceived group quality and the extent of being considered a group member.
The link between group success and maintaining a positive self-identity.
Identification and Ingroup Cooperation:
Identification Definition: The degree to which people merge their sense of self with the group.
Higher identification leads to:
Greater willingness to act cooperatively.
More investment in working toward group success (mandatory and discretionary behavior).
Competitive Behavior and Comparison Concerns:
Definition of Competition:
The presence of socially comparative groups triggers competitive behavior (Festinger, 1954).
Social comparison theory suggests individuals are driven to improve their performance and minimize discrepancies with others (Garcia et al., 2013).
Comparison Concerns:
Prime Motivator:
Garcia et al. (2013) emphasize that comparison concerns drive competitive behavior.
Defined as the desire to achieve or maintain a 'superior relative position.'
Note: This notion implies a perception of a superior relative position rather than an absolute one.
Factors Influencing Comparison Concerns:
Relevance of Performance Dimension:
Tesser (1988) indicates that competitiveness increases with the relevance of the activity to the individual.
Degree of Similarity to the Target:
Kilduff et al. (2010) find that competitiveness rises with greater similarity to the rival.
Closeness to the Target:
Pleban and Tesser (1981) reveal that competitiveness increases with the proximity of the individual to their rival.
Definitions:
Prejudice: Unjustified, usually negative thoughts and feelings based on perceived group membership.
Discrimination: Negative behavior based on perceived group membership.
Components and Manifestation:
Prejudice has cognitive and affective components, while discrimination is the behavioral expression.
Prejudice doesn't always translate into behavior; individuals may hold prejudicial views without acting on them.
Challenges in Studying:
Social desirability bias can impact the self-reporting of prejudicial attitudes.
Groups are dynamic, and individuals may identify with multiple groups, complicating research.
Prejudice Factors:
Taylor and Moriarty (1987) found competition and physical distinctiveness amplify ingroup favoritism and outgroup prejudice.
The Stereotype Content Model (SCM) identifies warmth and competence as fundamental dimensions in ingroup/outgroup perceptions.
Discrimination Factors:
Discrimination's behavioral nature requires a sociocultural context for manifestation.
Etieyibo and Omiegbe (2016) explored cultural and religious contributions to discrimination against people with disabilities in Nigeria.
Discrimination Forms in Nigeria:
Discrimination includes serious human rights abuses, such as burning women accused of witchcraft, raping women with mental illness for perceived wealth, and killing people with albinism for rituals.
Cultural Influence on Discrimination:
Disability is viewed as a cultural construct, and discriminatory practices are rooted in cultural beliefs.
People with disabilities are targeted for perceived social, personal, and economic benefits, leading to ritualized killings.
Religion and Culture Impact:
Religion and culture play powerful roles in shaping negative attitudes and discriminatory practices against people with disabilities in Nigeria.
Global Issue:
Discrimination against people with disabilities is not limited to Nigeria; historically, it has been prevalent globally, marked by various oppressive social attitudes.
Introduction:
Definition: Peace psychology is an academic field focused on understanding and addressing conflicts within groups, aiming to achieve sustainable peace through non-violent methods.
Scope: Encompasses psychological dynamics, politics, diplomacy, military and economic considerations, and sociocultural, international, and national structures affecting peace.
Biological Perspectives:
Inclusive fitness and kin selection theory suggest instinctive ingroup favoritism, fostering intergroup competition and intragroup cooperation.
Sociocultural Dynamics:
Social Identity Theory (SIT) by Tajfel emphasizes the natural division of the social environment into ingroups and outgroups.
Social categorization reduces perceived differences within ingroups and exaggerates differences between ingroups and outgroups (category accentuation effect).
Ethnocentrism, an ingroup-serving bias, can be explained by SIT.
Cognitive Perspectives:
Social Comparison: Individuals enhance self-esteem by positively perceiving their ingroup and negatively perceiving other groups.
Taylor and Moriarty (1987) demonstrate how physical distinctiveness along racial lines plays a crucial role in conflict origins.
Stereotype Content Model (SCM):
SCM shows the correlation between perceived status of outgroups, perceived competence, and competition. Outgroups are seen as warm when not competing with ingroups.
Social Dominance Orientation (SDO):
A personality trait where individuals prefer maintaining or increasing group differences, indicating a preference for hierarchies in social systems.
Identifiable Ingroups and Outgroups: The presence of distinct group identities contributes to conflict.
Ethnocentricity: Belief in the superiority of the ingroup over the outgroup.
Dehumanization: Treating the outgroup as less than human.
Legitimization of Violence: Acceptance of violence towards the outgroup.
Enemy Images: Promotion of negative perceptions of the outgroup.
Education System Influence: Presence of educational values promoting power differentials, authoritarianism, and Social Dominance Orientation (SDO).
Recognition of Group Allegiance: Acknowledging that group allegiance can lead to intergroup conflict.
Understanding Group Identity: Recognizing the power of group identity, often referred to as the 'collective self.'
Building Peace Structures: Establishing and maintaining structures that promote peace and social responsibility, including physical and cultural aspects.
Objective: Investigate the rate of helping behavior in different cities globally, considering economic development and cultural factors, including simpatia.
Methodology: Recorded instances of helping in various situations (e.g., a person pretending to be blind, a person with a leg brace dropping magazines) in major cities from 23 countries.
Findings:
Inverse Relationship with Economic Development: Countries with lower economic productivity were generally more helpful.
Role of Simpatia: Countries with a cultural tradition of simpatia were, on average, more helpful than those without.
Explanation: Poorer communities may exhibit higher levels of helping due to increased group cohesion.
Cohesive Groups: People experience less anxiety and tension in more cohesive groups (Myers, 1962).
Stress Coping: Belonging to a cohesive group contributes to better stress coping (Zaccaro et al., 1995).
Prosocial Behavior: Actions that benefit others or have positive social consequences.
Altruism: Motivational state with the ultimate goal of increasing another's welfare (Batson, 1991).
Critique: Some argue that all human behavior ultimately seeks personal pleasure, challenging the existence of true altruism.
Cultural Influence: Bystanderism studies consider economic development and cultural notions (e.g., simpatia).
Diffusion of Responsibility: Bystander behavior is influenced by the number of people present (Darley & Latané, 1968).
Social Identity Theory: People naturally categorize the social world into groups, influencing prosocial behavior.
Decision-Making Process (Latané & Darley, 1970):
Notice the situation.
Appraise it as an emergency.
Develop feelings of responsibility.
Believe in having the skills to succeed.
Reach a conscious decision to help.
Empathy-Altruism Model (Batson, 1991):
Empathetic concern: Tenderness, compassion, sympathy.
Emotional and motivational components influence prosocial behavior.
Evolutionary Perspective: Genes are central, promoting survival and reproduction.
Inclusive Fitness: Ability to pass on genes to the next generation, considering direct and shared genes of close relatives.
Kin Selection Theory: Prosocial behavior as an adaptive response for increasing group and individual survival.
Empirical Support: Studies with animals (e.g., vampire bats, squirrels) and humans (Madsen et al., 2007) show kin selection in action.
Circumstantial Cues: Biological relatedness moderated by shared developmental environment, familiarity, and social bonding (Sherman et al., 1997).
Empathy-Altruism: Cognitive focus, internal psychological processes.
Limited applicability to animal research due to the lack of cognitive insight.
Focus on day-to-day events rather than extreme situations.
Kin Selection: Evolutionary theory focuses on biological reasons for altruistic behavior.
Assumes animals' behavior informs understanding of human behavior.
Focus on extreme events threatening individual and group survival.
Complementary Nature: Both theories can complement each other, addressing different aspects of prosocial behavior.
Cognitive processes outlined in empathy-altruism likely result from evolutionary processes.
Prosocial Behavior: Behavior benefiting others or having positive social consequences (Staub, 1978).
Parental Influence: Parents play a crucial role; warm parenting is associated with positive outcomes.
Discipline Styles: Harsh discipline is negatively related to prosocial behavior; warmth, attachment, and responsiveness foster prosocial outcomes.
Compassion and Regulation: Compassion motivates prosocial behavior; regulation of personal emotions is crucial.
Negative Emotions: Individuals prone to negative emotions are less likely to engage in prosocial behavior.
Temperamental Fearfulness: Influences a child's ability to internalize societal standards for prosocial behavior.
Temperamental Fearfulness: Biologically influenced trait impacting prosocial behavior.
Inclusive Fitness: Evolutionary perspective; genes promoting prosocial behavior for group survival.
Kin Selection Theory: Explains altruistic social behavior via genetic relatedness and shared genes.
Kochanska's Framework (1993): Internal morality and internalization of societal standards.
Conscience Conceptualization: Two constructs - moral emotions and rule-compatible conduct.
Interaction of Anxiety and Fearlessness: Anxiety interacts with parenting and social norms; empathy may be hindered by personal distress.
a. Kindness Curriculum in Preschool Children (Flook et al., 2015):
Objective: Evaluate the effectiveness of a 12-week mindfulness-based Kindness Curriculum (KC).
Components: Mindfulness, prosocial skills training, empathy, gratitude, and sharing.
Results: KC group showed larger gains in social competence; and lower selfish behavior compared to control.
b. Emotion Coaching (Rose et al., 2015):
Objective: Test the effectiveness of emotion coaching on children's behavior.
Key Elements: Empathy and guidance; recognizing and validating emotions; problem-solving for self-regulation.
Results: Improved communication, reduced disruptive behavior, and increased prosocial behavior in children.
Quantitative Data: Reduced callouts in class, decreased exclusions, and improved well-being for adults.
Participants actively engaged in relationship research may have expectations affecting their behavior.
Reactivity, where participants modify behavior to please researchers, can introduce bias.
Participants may exhibit social desirability bias, presenting themselves in socially acceptable ways.
Research methods must account for potential biases in understanding prejudice and discrimination.
Researchers must be reflexive, acknowledging personal biases and contributions to meaning construction.
Awareness of background, beliefs, and potential biases crucial for minimizing research distortions.
Relationship issues are complex, requiring a combination of qualitative and quantitative mixed methods.
Triangulation, using multiple data sources and perspectives, enhances research validity.
Tyler and Blader (2001): Measured the influence of identification on cooperation with groups using anonymous questionnaires.
Puts (2005): Explored the effect of ovulation on attractiveness of male voices, revealing a link between ovulation cycles and perceived attractiveness.
Fiske et al. (2002): Investigated stereotypes, emphasizing careful selection of outgroups to reduce social desirability concerns.
Representational generalization involves applying findings to populations beyond the study's scope.
Theoretical generalization extends theoretical concepts from the study to develop further theory.
Researchers must consider the uniqueness of the populations studied and the relevance of findings to wider social theories.
Voluntary Participation:
Participants must be informed that their participation is voluntary.
Researchers should clarify that participants can withdraw consent at any stage.
Providing Sufficient Information:
Participants should receive adequate information about the study.
Details include study funding, conductors, data usage, time requirements, and study topics.
Obligation and Relationships:
Special consideration when researchers have a relationship with participants (e.g., counselor, professor).
Awareness of potential feelings of obligation and ensuring voluntary participation.
Consent Renegotiation:
Participants should understand that consent can be renegotiated.
Flexibility in consent is crucial, especially when unexpected circumstances arise.
Involvement of Children:
When children under 16 are involved, consent must be obtained from parents or legal guardians.
Respecting individual decisions while prioritizing the child's well-being.
Use of Deception:
Researchers may use mild deception, but it should be justified.
Ethical concerns arise, and potential harm should be minimized.
Example: Flook et al. (2015):
In the study testing the effectiveness of the Kindness Curriculum, informed consent was obtained from parents.
Children were individually consulted, respecting their decisions.
Example: Aronson and Linder (1965):
Used deception to study the effect of overheard compliments on male attraction.
Raised ethical concerns due to the potential harm caused by manipulating self-esteem.
Preventive Action in Sensitive Research
Researchers must take preventive action to avoid harm, especially in sensitive topics like relationship problems or prejudice.
Qualitative methods may lead participants to disclose private information; precautions are necessary.
Sensitivity to Participants' Feelings
Researchers in human relationships must be sensitive to participants' feelings.
Consideration of potential emotional impact and ethical concerns is crucial.
Example: Kiesler and Baral (1970)
Tested the influence of self-esteem on interactions with an attractive woman.
Modern ethical standards would question deliberately manipulating self-esteem.
Clear Understanding and Direct Questions
Participants should have a clear understanding of research topics before participation.
Researchers should use clear and direct questions to avoid unnecessary details and discomfort.
Empathy and Discontinuation
If participants show discomfort, researchers should be empathetic and consider discontinuing the research.
Prioritize participants' well-being and emotional comfort.
Avoiding Advice and Providing Information
Researchers should avoid providing advice or counseling participants.
Information about where to find help can be provided if needed.
Anonymity and Confidentiality
Essential in psychological research to protect participants.
Identity should be unknown outside the research team, and measures must be in place to ensure confidentiality.
Guaranteeing Anonymity
Researchers must demonstrate to RECs how they will guarantee participant anonymity.
Data should not be identifiable to readers, and anonymity should be maintained in report details.
Confidentiality Assurance
Ensuring that research data remains confidential.
Archiving practices should align with confidentiality standards, and non-anonymized data should only be archived with participant consent.
Ethical Issues in Small-Scale Research
Small-scale research, like case studies, poses challenges to anonymity.
Researchers must address the risk of participant identification in reports.
Definition of Culture
Culture encompasses attitudes, behaviors, and symbols shared by a large group and passed down through generations.
Shiraev and Levy (2004) define culture as a set of beliefs, values, superstitions, stereotypes, norms, customs, traditions, and fashions.
Sociocultural Events Influence Relationships:
Sociocultural events shape individuals' preferences in forming relationships.
Cultural dynamics impact who individuals are inclined to build connections with.
Components of Culture:
Attitudes:
Beliefs, encompassing political, religious, and moral perspectives.
Values that guide individuals' priorities and principles.
Superstitions and stereotypes influence perceptions.
Behaviors:
Norms dictate acceptable actions within a society.
Customs and traditions shape social interactions.
Fashions influence lifestyle choices and preferences.
Impact on Perception of Romantic Love:
Cultural norms and values influence how romantic love is perceived.
Defines societal expectations regarding the intensity and expression of romantic feelings.
Criteria for Attraction:
Cultural norms dictate what qualities are deemed attractive in a potential partner.
Influences preferences related to physical appearance, social status, and educational background.
Guidance on Relationship Initiation:
Cultural norms guide when individuals are expected to initiate romantic relationships.
Societal expectations regarding the appropriate timing for entering into romantic relations.
Role in Partner Selection:
Cultural values impact the characteristics considered desirable in a partner.
Shapes individuals' criteria for selecting a mate, including factors like personality, values, and social status.
Arranged Marriages and Cultural Influence:
Some cultures practice arranged marriages, emphasizing the role of societal expectations in relationship formation.
Reflects the influence of cultural norms on familial involvement in selecting partners.
Gender Roles and Relationship Dynamics:
Cultural expectations regarding gender roles impact power dynamics and division of labor within relationships.
Societal attitudes toward evolving gender roles can influence the nature of relationships.
Cultural Diversity in Relationships:
Sociocultural factors are crucial in intercultural relationships.
Managing cultural differences becomes a central aspect of relationship formation.
Cross-Cultural Differences in Attractiveness:
Cunningham et al. (1995) observed cultural variations in the perception of attractive bodies.
Example: African American men found larger women more attractive than white men, highlighting cultural specificity in beauty standards.
Cross-Cultural Variances in Female Physical Attributes:
Swami et al. (2006) compared UK and Japanese perceptions of an attractive female body.
Findings: Japanese preferences differed significantly, emphasizing lower body mass indexes (BMI) and specific body shapes compared to Britons.
BMI as a Universal Determinant:
Swami and Tovee (2005) found BMI as the primary determinant of female physical attractiveness in both the UK and Malaysia.
Cultural Variation: Attractiveness preferences varied along a socioeconomic gradient, with urban participants favoring lower BMIs compared to rural counterparts.
Buss's Cross-Cultural Study on Relationship Formation:
Buss (1994) investigated factors in relationship formation across 37 cultures.
Universals: Men prefer younger mates, and women prefer older mates.
Cultural Differences: Varied importance placed on characteristics; e.g., virginity is highly valued in China, Taiwan, India, and Iran, but less so in Finland, Denmark, Sweden, France, and Germany.
Impact of Rural vs. Urban Contexts:
The importance of love in relationship formation may vary with the level of urbanization and survival pressures.
Lindholm's suggestion: In more rural communities where survival is a primary concern, love may be less important compared to other factors.
Globalization and Changing Cultural Constructs:
The spread of Western approaches to relationships worldwide is noted, possibly influenced by globalization.
Romantic love, while not universal, is becoming more widespread as cultures adopt aspects of Western relationship ideals.
Passionate Love as Culturally Specific:
Goodwin (1995) argues that the notion of passionate love as a primary reason for forming serious relationships is culturally specific, particularly to Western cultures.
In more traditional societies, the emphasis is on loving the person one marries rather than marrying for passionate love.
Arranged Marriages as Sociocultural Norms:
In several cultures, arranged marriages are common, challenging the Western emphasis on passionate love as a basis for marriage.
Parents often play a central role in the decision-making process, sometimes presenting a range of potential partners for their child.
Lack of Individual Choice in Arranged Marriages:
In some cases of arranged marriages, the individuals involved may have little or no say in choosing their partners or deciding on the marriage.
The decision-making power often lies with parents or other family members.
Factors Contributing to Arranged Marriage Success:
In arranged marriages, the involvement of a third party (e.g., parents) is considered a factor contributing to their success.
Intellectual elements, along with emotional factors, play a role in decision-making.
Definition of Social Cues:
Social cues are visual or non-verbal characteristics in an environment that influence people's thoughts, emotions, and behaviors.
Examples of Social Cues:
Body Language:
Gestures, postures, and movements that convey information without verbal communication.
Example: A person crossing their arms may indicate defensiveness or resistance.
Facial Expressions:
Emotional expressions are conveyed through changes in facial muscles.
Example: A smile indicates happiness or approval, while a frown may convey disapproval or sadness.
Assumptions:
Biological Influence on Relationship Formation:
Assumes that biological events significantly influence individuals' preferences in forming relationships.
Survival Purpose of Heterosexual Relationships:
Explains that heterosexual relationships serve a survival purpose by facilitating the transmission of the best possible genetic combinations to offspring.
Evolutionary Arguments for Heterosexuality:
Attraction as an Adaptive Function:
Attraction is viewed as a product of evolution, serving as an adaptive function for the survival and reproduction of the species.
Gender-Specific Preferences:
Males:
Generally prefer younger women.
Pay attention to physical details like teeth, lip color, hair length and shine, hip size, and skin smoothness—characteristics representing the ability to produce offspring.
Females:
Generally prefer older men.
Pay attention to characteristics like ambition, wealth, intelligence, social status, energy levels, and good health—traits associated with the ability to provide for offspring.
Genetic Point of View:
Attraction, according to this perspective, has evolved as a genetically encoded trait.
Similarities and Differences:
Similarities:
Both genders pay attention to the appearance of a potential mate.
Both sexes value traits like 'kindness' and 'intelligence.'
Differences:
Males and females exhibit gender-specific preferences based on evolutionary needs for reproductive success.
Empirical Support:
The gender differences in attraction are supported by large-scale cross-cultural surveys, such as those conducted by Buss (1994, 1995).
Assumption:
The attraction between individuals is influenced by the combination of their genes, to produce healthy offspring.
Evidence for Assumption:
Attraction to pheromones carried in sweat, containing information about a person's immune system.
Assumption: Genetically different immune systems complement each other, potentially resulting in offspring with a robust immune system.
Role of MHC Genes:
Control of Immunological Self on-Self Discrimination:
MHC genes control immunological self-self discrimination, tissue rejection, and immune recognition of infectious diseases.
Importance for Health:
MHC genes play a crucial role in ensuring individuals stay healthy by regulating immune responses.
Sexual Selection and MHC Genes:
Avoiding Inbreeding:
MHC genes are assumed to be the product of sexual selection, aiming to improve the immune system of offspring and prevent inbreeding.
Studies on Mice:
Studies in house mice suggest that both males and females prefer mates with MHC-dissimilar genes, recognized through odor cues.
Studies in Humans:
Human studies, including those by Wedekind and Penn (2000), found associations between MHC-associated odors and mating preferences.
Cognitive Health Connection:
Facial symmetry is linked to greater cognitive health, with symmetry indicating the reliability and precision of developmental processes (Penke et al., 2009).
Supports the bodily integrity hypothesis, suggesting a correlation between scoring well on cognitive ability tests and a general tendency for overall body systems to be efficient and healthy (Deary, 2012).
Associations with Physical Health:
Facial symmetry is not only an indicator of cognitive health but is also associated with better physical health.
The bodily cues provided by facial symmetry may offer insights into both cognitive and physical well-being.
The Golden Ratio:
Facial symmetry has been suggested to be linked with the mathematical concept of the 'golden ratio.'
The golden ratio refers to pleasing and harmonious proportions that adhere to a specific mathematical equation.
The complexity of Achieving Symmetry:
Achieving facial symmetry is a complex task during human growth, involving billions of cell reproductions while maintaining a parallel structure.
Facial symmetry is considered a visible signal of genetic health, as it reflects successful and precise developmental processes.
Biological Origins and Cross-Cultural Consistency:
Perceiving facial symmetry has a clear cognitive element and is cross-cultural, suggesting biological origins.
The universality of appreciating facial symmetry across cultures indicates a shared, biologically rooted perception.
Limitations:
While facial symmetry is considered an indicator of attractiveness, there are cross-cultural differences in beauty standards.
Cultural factors can influence what is perceived as beautiful, emphasizing the interplay between biological and cultural influences.
Impact of Ovulation on Women's Attractiveness:
Ovulation is believed to influence women's preferences in men and their physical appearance, impacting their attractiveness to prospective partners.
Concealed Ovulation in Humans:
Unlike some other mammals, human females conceal ovulation, giving them more control over reproduction.
Concealing ovulation allows females to have more control and potentially cheat on mates without being detected during ovulation.
Definition:
The false correlation of positive characteristics, assuming that someone with a few positive traits possesses many positive characteristics.
Association with Physical Attractiveness:
The halo effect is sometimes linked to physical attractiveness, where people may assume that physically attractive individuals possess other positive traits.
Research Context:
Ahmad and Reid (2008) explored the connection between traditional roles, communication styles, and marital satisfaction.
Focused on South Asian couples in Canada, particularly those with traditional beliefs in marriage.
Methodology:
Married individuals of South Asian descent participated.
Only one partner per couple completed the questionnaire.
Questionnaire Content:
Explored attitudes toward marriage.
Examined communication styles within the marriage.
Assessed marital satisfaction.
Key Findings:
Traditional role beliefs did not necessarily lead to lower satisfaction.
The critical factor was the listening style in the relationship.
Listening Style and Satisfaction:
Couples with traditional beliefs but effective listening were more likely to be satisfied.
Emphasizes the importance of actively listening to understand a partner's feelings.
Insights from Arranged Marriages:
Arranged marriages with traditional beliefs can be satisfying.
Listening plays a pivotal role in producing satisfaction despite having fewer choices in partners.
Implications:
Communication, specifically active listening, may play a more significant role in relationship satisfaction than cultural factors influencing marriage.
Explanation of Behavior: Attribution involves explaining the behavior of others in our environment.
Emotional Influence: Attributions can be influenced by our emotional connection and feelings toward the person.
Social Exchange Theory (SET):
Basic Assumption: Relationships are maintained through continuous cost-benefit analysis.
Balance Requirement: Balance between partners' investments and returns is crucial for relationship stability.
Dependence on Perception: SET involves the perception of imbalances and inequality, which is subjective to individual viewpoints.
Student A study Carlson et al. (2014):
Focus: Analyzed data on housework and sexual frequency from the 2006 Marital and Relationship Survey.
Participants: 600 married and cohabiting low to moderate-income couples with children.
Findings:
Equal Housework and Sexual Frequency:
Couples sharing routine housework equally reported the highest sexual frequency (7.74 times a month).
Higher satisfaction and quality of the sexual relationship in these couples.
Gender Equality and Intimacy:
Changing Gender Roles: Men participating in housework and childcare are seen as fair.
Increased Intimacy: Perception of fairness in cost-benefit analysis leads to increased intimacy in Western societies.
Role of Expectations:
Traditional vs. Non-Traditional Expectations:
Traditional marriages may have different expectations regarding roles and contributions.
Clear roles in traditional settings may provide a framework for perceived fairness.
Norway Divorce Rates:
Traditional Roles vs. Gender Equality:
Despite Norway's gender equality, couples with more traditional roles had lower divorce rates.
Suggests that clarity in roles and expectations aligns with perceived fairness.
Sociocultural changes, particularly in the West, have led to increased empowerment and financial independence for women.
Shifts in cultural norms and economic structures have influenced women's roles in the workforce and relationships.
Statistical Trends (Office for National Statistics, 2013):
The employment rate of women aged 16 to 64 rose from 53% in 1971 to 66% in 2012.
Concurrently, there has been a decline in employment for men.
Factors Influencing Sociocultural Change:
Rise of the Service Sector:
Shift from heavy industry to the service sector, favoring opportunities for women.
Women actively developing careers in diverse fields.
Impact on Relationships:
Career development requires time and energy, potentially delaying serious or long-term relationships.
Pressure on women due to biological time constraints related to fertility.
Men, not facing the same fertility decline, may feel less urgency to commit in the early stages of a relationship.
Relationship Dynamics: Early vs. Later Stages:
Early Stages:
Women may feel pressure to manage both careers and the desire to have children.
Men may perceive less urgency regarding commitment due to a lack of similar biological time constraints.
Later Stages:
Increased financial independence for women.
Accumulation of financial capital, such as shared property ownership.
Women are more capable of supporting themselves outside marriage.
Impact on Freedom of Choice:
Women have more freedom of choice in later relationship stages:
Financial independence.
Easier and cheaper divorce options.
Less Stigma for Divorce in Western Cultures
Introduction of No-Fault Divorce:
Initiated in some US states in the 1950s.
Couples were no longer required to prove fault; citing a broken-down marriage was sufficient.
By 1970, most US states adopted laws allowing no-fault divorces.
Impact on Divorce Rates:
Divorce Rates in the US:
1940-1965: The divorce rate remained near ten divorces for every 1000 married women.
1979: The rate increased to 20 divorces for every 1000 married women.
Factors Contributing to Increased Divorce Rates:
Adoption of no-fault divorce laws.
Changing societal attitudes toward divorce.
Changing Societal Influences
Decline of Religious Influence:
Religion's diminished role in influencing relationship decisions.
Couples are more likely to seek divorce when facing challenges.
Legal Relaxations:
Legal changes making divorce more accessible.
Impact on Stigma
Reduced Stigma:
No-fault divorce reduces the stigma associated with divorce.
Couples can choose divorce without attributing blame to one party.
Attitude Shifts:
Changing societal attitudes towards divorce.
Divorce becomes a more acceptable option for couples facing challenges.
Increased Life Expectancy
Health Improvements:
Better healthcare and education contribute to longer, healthier lives.
The overall increase in life expectancy in Western developed economies.
Changing Perspectives:
People no longer view late middle age as the end of life.
A shift towards starting second major relationships in extended life cycles.
Impact on Sexual Health
STD Rates:
As couples end long-term relationships, STD rates among older individuals increase.
In the USA (2013), people aged 55 and older accounted for 26% of those living with HIV.
Medication and Sexual Activity:
Introduction of medications for erectile dysfunction for older men.
Enables older individuals to engage in sexual activity in later years.
Lack of Education:
Older men tend to be less educated about STD risks.
May not be well-informed about safe practices.
Perceived Low Risk:
Older individuals may perceive themselves at low risk for STDs.
Contributes to the spread of STDs in this age group.
Inclusive Fitness
Definition:
The ability of an individual organism to pass on its genes to the next generation.
Consider direct descendants and shared genes of close relatives.
Explanation:
Enhances evolutionary success by promoting the reproduction and survival of genetically similar individuals.
Underpins kin selection theory, emphasizing intragroup cooperation as an adaptation.
Intragroup Cooperation and Kin Selection
Behavioral Adaptations:
Individuals engage in behaviors benefiting their ingroup, even at personal cost.
Motivation to help genetically similar individuals within the group.
Kin selection theory suggests intragroup cooperation as an evolutionary adaptation.
Intergroup Competitiveness
Alternative Adaptation:
Intergroup competitiveness is considered an adaptation.
Non-human primates exhibit sensitivity to social hierarchies and group distinctions.
Competitiveness is observed in more primitive species, responding to situational factors.
Examples from Non-Human Primates:
Primate Behavior:
Non-human primates show sensitivity to individual rankings within groups.
Recognition of social category fault lines, distinguishing between ingroup and outgroups.
Primitive Species and Competitiveness:
Sensitivity to Situational Factors:
Primitive species, such as beetle larvae and cockroaches, exhibit competitiveness.
Sensitivity to situational factors, including the number of potential competitors.
Conceptual Framework
Developed by Tajfel and Turner (1979).
Assumes natural division of social environment into ingroups (us) and outgroups (them or the other).
Social Categorization:
Reduction of Differences:
Reduces perceived differences within the ingroup.
Reduces perceived differences in the outgroup.
Increases perceived differences between the ingroup and outgroup.
Category Accentuation Effect:
Exaggeration of Differences:
Exaggeration of ingroup and outgroup differences.
Accentuation of intragroup similarities.
Social Identity Process:
Assimilation:
Individuals assimilate into their group by adopting the group's identity.
Behave in ways consistent with group behavior.
The group becomes the person's ingroup.
Social Comparison:
Identification and Comparison:
Individuals categorize themselves within a group.
Compare their ingroup with another group (outgroup).
Positive distinctiveness for self-esteem enhancement.
Ethnocentrism:
Ingroup Bias:
Positive behaviors of ingroup attributed to dispositional factors.
Negative behaviors of ingroup attributed to situational factors.
Positive behaviors of outgroup attributed to situational factors.
Negative behaviors of outgroup attributed to dispositional factors.
Stereotyping:
Fixed Images:
Fixed and oversimplified images of individuals or groups.
Occurs when individuals are perceived as members of an outgroup.
Assumes shared characteristics among outgroup members.
Perceptions of Group Homogeneity:
Linked to Social Identity:
Stereotypes are influenced by social identity processes.
Group homogeneity perceptions apply to both ingroups and outgroups.
Group Dynamics:
Definition:
System of behaviors and psychological processes within social ingroups.
Study applicable to understanding prosocial behavior, prejudice, discrimination, and violence.
Intragroup and Intergroup Dynamics:
Ingroups and outgroups are defined through comparisons and contrasts.
Cooperation:
Definition: A prosocial process where individuals work together for mutual benefit.
Contrast with Competition:
Competition: Individuals work toward selfish, non-shareable goals.
Cooperation usually occurs within ingroups as an intragroup process.
Enhancement of Organized Groups:
Shared group identity increases cooperative behavior (Brewer, 2008).
The viability of organized groups is boosted when members act to enhance group functioning (Tyler & Blader, 2000).
Tyler and Blader's Types of Cooperation Within Ingroups:
Following Group Rules:
Conducting Work on Behalf of the Group:
Intention to Stay Within the Group:
Intention has a cognitive nature but also involves social signaling.
A lack of clear intention may be interpreted as a lack of motivation to cooperate.
Two Types of Basic Cooperative Behavior:
Mandatory (Required) Behaviors:
Specified for particular roles in a group.
Originates from the group or group leader.
Discretionary (Non-Required) Behaviors:
Goes beyond required actions.
Originates from a group member, not the group.
SIT's Role in Ingroup Cooperation:
Individuals cooperate with their group as long as it provides a positive social identity.
If not, individuals might be tempted to change group membership (Doosje et al., 1999).
Ongoing cognitive process: Constantly measuring perceived group quality and the extent of being considered a group member.
The link between group success and maintaining a positive self-identity.
Identification and Ingroup Cooperation:
Identification Definition: The degree to which people merge their sense of self with the group.
Higher identification leads to:
Greater willingness to act cooperatively.
More investment in working toward group success (mandatory and discretionary behavior).
Competitive Behavior and Comparison Concerns:
Definition of Competition:
The presence of socially comparative groups triggers competitive behavior (Festinger, 1954).
Social comparison theory suggests individuals are driven to improve their performance and minimize discrepancies with others (Garcia et al., 2013).
Comparison Concerns:
Prime Motivator:
Garcia et al. (2013) emphasize that comparison concerns drive competitive behavior.
Defined as the desire to achieve or maintain a 'superior relative position.'
Note: This notion implies a perception of a superior relative position rather than an absolute one.
Factors Influencing Comparison Concerns:
Relevance of Performance Dimension:
Tesser (1988) indicates that competitiveness increases with the relevance of the activity to the individual.
Degree of Similarity to the Target:
Kilduff et al. (2010) find that competitiveness rises with greater similarity to the rival.
Closeness to the Target:
Pleban and Tesser (1981) reveal that competitiveness increases with the proximity of the individual to their rival.
Definitions:
Prejudice: Unjustified, usually negative thoughts and feelings based on perceived group membership.
Discrimination: Negative behavior based on perceived group membership.
Components and Manifestation:
Prejudice has cognitive and affective components, while discrimination is the behavioral expression.
Prejudice doesn't always translate into behavior; individuals may hold prejudicial views without acting on them.
Challenges in Studying:
Social desirability bias can impact the self-reporting of prejudicial attitudes.
Groups are dynamic, and individuals may identify with multiple groups, complicating research.
Prejudice Factors:
Taylor and Moriarty (1987) found competition and physical distinctiveness amplify ingroup favoritism and outgroup prejudice.
The Stereotype Content Model (SCM) identifies warmth and competence as fundamental dimensions in ingroup/outgroup perceptions.
Discrimination Factors:
Discrimination's behavioral nature requires a sociocultural context for manifestation.
Etieyibo and Omiegbe (2016) explored cultural and religious contributions to discrimination against people with disabilities in Nigeria.
Discrimination Forms in Nigeria:
Discrimination includes serious human rights abuses, such as burning women accused of witchcraft, raping women with mental illness for perceived wealth, and killing people with albinism for rituals.
Cultural Influence on Discrimination:
Disability is viewed as a cultural construct, and discriminatory practices are rooted in cultural beliefs.
People with disabilities are targeted for perceived social, personal, and economic benefits, leading to ritualized killings.
Religion and Culture Impact:
Religion and culture play powerful roles in shaping negative attitudes and discriminatory practices against people with disabilities in Nigeria.
Global Issue:
Discrimination against people with disabilities is not limited to Nigeria; historically, it has been prevalent globally, marked by various oppressive social attitudes.
Introduction:
Definition: Peace psychology is an academic field focused on understanding and addressing conflicts within groups, aiming to achieve sustainable peace through non-violent methods.
Scope: Encompasses psychological dynamics, politics, diplomacy, military and economic considerations, and sociocultural, international, and national structures affecting peace.
Biological Perspectives:
Inclusive fitness and kin selection theory suggest instinctive ingroup favoritism, fostering intergroup competition and intragroup cooperation.
Sociocultural Dynamics:
Social Identity Theory (SIT) by Tajfel emphasizes the natural division of the social environment into ingroups and outgroups.
Social categorization reduces perceived differences within ingroups and exaggerates differences between ingroups and outgroups (category accentuation effect).
Ethnocentrism, an ingroup-serving bias, can be explained by SIT.
Cognitive Perspectives:
Social Comparison: Individuals enhance self-esteem by positively perceiving their ingroup and negatively perceiving other groups.
Taylor and Moriarty (1987) demonstrate how physical distinctiveness along racial lines plays a crucial role in conflict origins.
Stereotype Content Model (SCM):
SCM shows the correlation between perceived status of outgroups, perceived competence, and competition. Outgroups are seen as warm when not competing with ingroups.
Social Dominance Orientation (SDO):
A personality trait where individuals prefer maintaining or increasing group differences, indicating a preference for hierarchies in social systems.
Identifiable Ingroups and Outgroups: The presence of distinct group identities contributes to conflict.
Ethnocentricity: Belief in the superiority of the ingroup over the outgroup.
Dehumanization: Treating the outgroup as less than human.
Legitimization of Violence: Acceptance of violence towards the outgroup.
Enemy Images: Promotion of negative perceptions of the outgroup.
Education System Influence: Presence of educational values promoting power differentials, authoritarianism, and Social Dominance Orientation (SDO).
Recognition of Group Allegiance: Acknowledging that group allegiance can lead to intergroup conflict.
Understanding Group Identity: Recognizing the power of group identity, often referred to as the 'collective self.'
Building Peace Structures: Establishing and maintaining structures that promote peace and social responsibility, including physical and cultural aspects.
Objective: Investigate the rate of helping behavior in different cities globally, considering economic development and cultural factors, including simpatia.
Methodology: Recorded instances of helping in various situations (e.g., a person pretending to be blind, a person with a leg brace dropping magazines) in major cities from 23 countries.
Findings:
Inverse Relationship with Economic Development: Countries with lower economic productivity were generally more helpful.
Role of Simpatia: Countries with a cultural tradition of simpatia were, on average, more helpful than those without.
Explanation: Poorer communities may exhibit higher levels of helping due to increased group cohesion.
Cohesive Groups: People experience less anxiety and tension in more cohesive groups (Myers, 1962).
Stress Coping: Belonging to a cohesive group contributes to better stress coping (Zaccaro et al., 1995).
Prosocial Behavior: Actions that benefit others or have positive social consequences.
Altruism: Motivational state with the ultimate goal of increasing another's welfare (Batson, 1991).
Critique: Some argue that all human behavior ultimately seeks personal pleasure, challenging the existence of true altruism.
Cultural Influence: Bystanderism studies consider economic development and cultural notions (e.g., simpatia).
Diffusion of Responsibility: Bystander behavior is influenced by the number of people present (Darley & Latané, 1968).
Social Identity Theory: People naturally categorize the social world into groups, influencing prosocial behavior.
Decision-Making Process (Latané & Darley, 1970):
Notice the situation.
Appraise it as an emergency.
Develop feelings of responsibility.
Believe in having the skills to succeed.
Reach a conscious decision to help.
Empathy-Altruism Model (Batson, 1991):
Empathetic concern: Tenderness, compassion, sympathy.
Emotional and motivational components influence prosocial behavior.
Evolutionary Perspective: Genes are central, promoting survival and reproduction.
Inclusive Fitness: Ability to pass on genes to the next generation, considering direct and shared genes of close relatives.
Kin Selection Theory: Prosocial behavior as an adaptive response for increasing group and individual survival.
Empirical Support: Studies with animals (e.g., vampire bats, squirrels) and humans (Madsen et al., 2007) show kin selection in action.
Circumstantial Cues: Biological relatedness moderated by shared developmental environment, familiarity, and social bonding (Sherman et al., 1997).
Empathy-Altruism: Cognitive focus, internal psychological processes.
Limited applicability to animal research due to the lack of cognitive insight.
Focus on day-to-day events rather than extreme situations.
Kin Selection: Evolutionary theory focuses on biological reasons for altruistic behavior.
Assumes animals' behavior informs understanding of human behavior.
Focus on extreme events threatening individual and group survival.
Complementary Nature: Both theories can complement each other, addressing different aspects of prosocial behavior.
Cognitive processes outlined in empathy-altruism likely result from evolutionary processes.
Prosocial Behavior: Behavior benefiting others or having positive social consequences (Staub, 1978).
Parental Influence: Parents play a crucial role; warm parenting is associated with positive outcomes.
Discipline Styles: Harsh discipline is negatively related to prosocial behavior; warmth, attachment, and responsiveness foster prosocial outcomes.
Compassion and Regulation: Compassion motivates prosocial behavior; regulation of personal emotions is crucial.
Negative Emotions: Individuals prone to negative emotions are less likely to engage in prosocial behavior.
Temperamental Fearfulness: Influences a child's ability to internalize societal standards for prosocial behavior.
Temperamental Fearfulness: Biologically influenced trait impacting prosocial behavior.
Inclusive Fitness: Evolutionary perspective; genes promoting prosocial behavior for group survival.
Kin Selection Theory: Explains altruistic social behavior via genetic relatedness and shared genes.
Kochanska's Framework (1993): Internal morality and internalization of societal standards.
Conscience Conceptualization: Two constructs - moral emotions and rule-compatible conduct.
Interaction of Anxiety and Fearlessness: Anxiety interacts with parenting and social norms; empathy may be hindered by personal distress.
a. Kindness Curriculum in Preschool Children (Flook et al., 2015):
Objective: Evaluate the effectiveness of a 12-week mindfulness-based Kindness Curriculum (KC).
Components: Mindfulness, prosocial skills training, empathy, gratitude, and sharing.
Results: KC group showed larger gains in social competence; and lower selfish behavior compared to control.
b. Emotion Coaching (Rose et al., 2015):
Objective: Test the effectiveness of emotion coaching on children's behavior.
Key Elements: Empathy and guidance; recognizing and validating emotions; problem-solving for self-regulation.
Results: Improved communication, reduced disruptive behavior, and increased prosocial behavior in children.
Quantitative Data: Reduced callouts in class, decreased exclusions, and improved well-being for adults.
Participants actively engaged in relationship research may have expectations affecting their behavior.
Reactivity, where participants modify behavior to please researchers, can introduce bias.
Participants may exhibit social desirability bias, presenting themselves in socially acceptable ways.
Research methods must account for potential biases in understanding prejudice and discrimination.
Researchers must be reflexive, acknowledging personal biases and contributions to meaning construction.
Awareness of background, beliefs, and potential biases crucial for minimizing research distortions.
Relationship issues are complex, requiring a combination of qualitative and quantitative mixed methods.
Triangulation, using multiple data sources and perspectives, enhances research validity.
Tyler and Blader (2001): Measured the influence of identification on cooperation with groups using anonymous questionnaires.
Puts (2005): Explored the effect of ovulation on attractiveness of male voices, revealing a link between ovulation cycles and perceived attractiveness.
Fiske et al. (2002): Investigated stereotypes, emphasizing careful selection of outgroups to reduce social desirability concerns.
Representational generalization involves applying findings to populations beyond the study's scope.
Theoretical generalization extends theoretical concepts from the study to develop further theory.
Researchers must consider the uniqueness of the populations studied and the relevance of findings to wider social theories.
Voluntary Participation:
Participants must be informed that their participation is voluntary.
Researchers should clarify that participants can withdraw consent at any stage.
Providing Sufficient Information:
Participants should receive adequate information about the study.
Details include study funding, conductors, data usage, time requirements, and study topics.
Obligation and Relationships:
Special consideration when researchers have a relationship with participants (e.g., counselor, professor).
Awareness of potential feelings of obligation and ensuring voluntary participation.
Consent Renegotiation:
Participants should understand that consent can be renegotiated.
Flexibility in consent is crucial, especially when unexpected circumstances arise.
Involvement of Children:
When children under 16 are involved, consent must be obtained from parents or legal guardians.
Respecting individual decisions while prioritizing the child's well-being.
Use of Deception:
Researchers may use mild deception, but it should be justified.
Ethical concerns arise, and potential harm should be minimized.
Example: Flook et al. (2015):
In the study testing the effectiveness of the Kindness Curriculum, informed consent was obtained from parents.
Children were individually consulted, respecting their decisions.
Example: Aronson and Linder (1965):
Used deception to study the effect of overheard compliments on male attraction.
Raised ethical concerns due to the potential harm caused by manipulating self-esteem.
Preventive Action in Sensitive Research
Researchers must take preventive action to avoid harm, especially in sensitive topics like relationship problems or prejudice.
Qualitative methods may lead participants to disclose private information; precautions are necessary.
Sensitivity to Participants' Feelings
Researchers in human relationships must be sensitive to participants' feelings.
Consideration of potential emotional impact and ethical concerns is crucial.
Example: Kiesler and Baral (1970)
Tested the influence of self-esteem on interactions with an attractive woman.
Modern ethical standards would question deliberately manipulating self-esteem.
Clear Understanding and Direct Questions
Participants should have a clear understanding of research topics before participation.
Researchers should use clear and direct questions to avoid unnecessary details and discomfort.
Empathy and Discontinuation
If participants show discomfort, researchers should be empathetic and consider discontinuing the research.
Prioritize participants' well-being and emotional comfort.
Avoiding Advice and Providing Information
Researchers should avoid providing advice or counseling participants.
Information about where to find help can be provided if needed.
Anonymity and Confidentiality
Essential in psychological research to protect participants.
Identity should be unknown outside the research team, and measures must be in place to ensure confidentiality.
Guaranteeing Anonymity
Researchers must demonstrate to RECs how they will guarantee participant anonymity.
Data should not be identifiable to readers, and anonymity should be maintained in report details.
Confidentiality Assurance
Ensuring that research data remains confidential.
Archiving practices should align with confidentiality standards, and non-anonymized data should only be archived with participant consent.
Ethical Issues in Small-Scale Research
Small-scale research, like case studies, poses challenges to anonymity.
Researchers must address the risk of participant identification in reports.