Neural Basis of Behavior & Phenotypic Plasticity – Key Vocabulary
Phenotypic Plasticity & Polyphenism
- Definition: Phenotypic plasticity = capacity of one genotype to express >1 phenotype depending on environmental cues.
- Graphical variants (Fig. 3.28)
- Lots of variation around a single mean
- Minimal variation around a single mean
- Discontinuous variation with two or more means
- Relationship hierarchy
- Polyphenism = special case of phenotypic plasticity (discrete morphs)
- May or may not underlie observed phenotypic variation; some variation is non-plastic.
- Zero plasticity condition: genotype → one phenotype regardless of environment.
Categories & Examples of Polyphenism
- Density-dependent polyphenism
- Socially-induced polyphenisms (common in fish)
- Food-induced polyphenisms
- Predator-induced polyphenisms
- Triggered by predator kairomones (chemical cues released by predator)
- Empirical data
- Rotifer spines: \frac{18}{59} spined morphs vs typical
- Barnacle aperture rotation: \frac{11}{43} rotated vs typical
- Mollusc thickened, “toothed” shell requires ≥50\% loss of typical morphs before predation stops
- Carp body depth expanded: \frac{30}{100} deep-bodied under predation
Behavioral Polymorphism & Supergenes
- Behavioral polymorphism
- Loss of ability to plastically regulate behavioral roles according to environment.
- Leads to fixed behavioral morphs within same species.
- Frequently controlled by supergenes.
- Supergene (review)
- Region of DNA containing many linked genes that collectively influence a complex trait (e.g., behavioral phenotype).
- Tight linkage prevents recombination → preserves adaptive gene combinations.
Interactive Poll Prompts (lecture engagement cues)
- Spectrum of foraging behaviors in rodents → ask students “What type of phenotypic plasticity is this?” (continuous/gradient type)
- “Describe a supergene.”
- “Give an example of other instincts.”
- “Write a type of stimulus an organism may detect.”
- Emotional check-in (“In one word describe how you feel about week 1”).
Neural Basis of Behavior (Chapter 4)
- Learning Objectives
- Describe neurological processes responding to external stimuli.
- Relate neural mechanisms to major hypotheses in Animal Behavior.
Basic Neuroanatomy
- Neuron = main cell of nervous system
- Dendrite → receives signals
- Axon → transmits signals away from cell body
- Cell body → nucleus & organelles; metabolic control
- Synapse → junction transmitting electrical impulses & neurotransmitters
- Neural circuit → interconnected group of neurons
- Signaling process
- Dendrites receive input (from other neurons or sensory receptors).
- If input exceeds threshold, action potential (AP) propagates down axon.
- At synapse, electrical AP converted to neurotransmitter release → continues signal in next cell.
- Key reminders
- Neurons send & receive; detect stimuli & mediate responses.
- Morphology & neurotransmitter type vary by location & function.
- Neuronal architecture is highly conserved among mammals.
Stimulus–Response Foundations (Tinbergen & Lorenz)
- Research questions: “What sensory input stimulates pecking?” (herring gull chicks – Tinbergen)
- Innate behavior (instinct)
- Fully formed on first performance; no learning required.
- Fixed Action Pattern (FAP)
- Once triggered by releaser (sign stimulus), sequence runs to completion.
- Mediated by Innate Releasing Mechanism (IRM) – dedicated neural circuit.
- Classic examples
- Egg-rolling in greylag geese (Lorenz)
- Male stickleback attack behavior triggered by red belly.
- Data snippet (Fig. 4.4)
- Relative pecking responses: 100, 92, 35, 126 across model variants.
Are Instincts Always Beneficial?
- Exploitation scenarios
- Blister beetle larvae aggregate to mimic female bee → male bee attempts mating, transports larvae to hive → larvae consume provisions.
- Avian brood parasitism (cuckoo vs reed warbler).
- Stimulus = gaping mouth markings & begging calls.
- Coevolutionary arms race in estrildid vs vidua finches (Fig. 4.6)
- Parasite chicks evolve host-like mouth markings.
- Hosts evolve finer discrimination of markings.
Sensory Modalities & Stimulus Filtering Overview
- Acoustic: moths vs bats
- Visual: monarch butterfly migration
- Tactile: star-nosed mole foraging
- Olfactory/Chemical: predator-induced plasticity, kairomones
- Stimulus filtering = neural capacity to ignore irrelevant data & prioritize biologically relevant cues; species-specific and can be automatic or modulated.
Acoustic Case Study: How Moths Avoid Bats
- Bats use ultrasonic echolocation pulses; moths evolved counter-measures.
- Moth ear anatomy (Noctuids)
- Tympanum on body surface; two receptor neurons: A1 (sensitive) & A2 (responds only to loud, close bats).
- Signal transduction (Fig. 4.7–4.9)
- Sound → tympanum vibration.
- Vibration → receptor neuron depolarization.
- Receptor AP frequency proportional to sound intensity (distance) and lateral differences (direction).
- Interneurons relay to motor neurons → wing muscle adjustments: dives/swerves.
- Direction coding (Fig. 4.10)
- \text{Left\,vs.\,Right A1 rate} \Rightarrow source direction.
- Tiger moth ultrasonic clicks
- Hypotheses tested (Corcoran et al. 2009):
- Startle effect
- Aposematic “I taste bad” warning
- Sonar jamming (strongly supported)
- Experiment: bats given silent vs clicking moths; measured attack success.
Visual Case Study: Monarch Butterfly Migration
- Migration map (Fig. 4.15): breeding range \approx 20^{\circ}N-50^{\circ}N; overwinter in Sierra Madre, Mexico.
- Sun compass navigation
- Day-flying migrants use sun azimuth; must compensate for sun’s movement using circadian clock.
- Circadian Clock Integration (Froy et al. 2003)
- Flight simulator experiments
- Normal LD cycle → correct south-westerly orientation (“Mexico!”).
- Clock advanced +6\,\text{h} → mis-oriented (east, over Atlantic).
- Conclusion: navigation = sun position × internal time reference.
- UV & Polarized Light Cues
- UV light required to initiate correct orientation.
- UV-filter in simulator → moths stop or orient randomly; removal restores direction.
- Polarized light patterns (sky polarization rotated 90^{\circ}) dictate heading corrections as sun moves.
- Neural mechanism (Fig. 4.15, 4.16)
- UV-sensitive receptors in antennae activate “clock” genes → brain → directional flight commands.
- Ocular photoreceptors detect sun angle; combined with clock outputs.
Tactile Case Study: Star-Nosed Mole Foraging
- Sensory organ: nose with 22 highly mobile rays containing ≈25\,000 Eimer’s organs (mechanoreceptors).
- Behavioral performance (Fig. 4.26)
- Contact to prey capture in 300–400\,\text{ms}; rays move independently.
- Neural representation (Catania & Kaas 1997)
- Somatosensory cortex has spatially distinct neuron clusters per ray.
- Cortical magnification: ray 11 (“tactile fovea”) occupies disproportionate cortical area.
- Comparative magnification across insectivores (Fig. 4.27).
Stimulus Filtering: Parasitoid Fly (Ormia ochracea)
- Life history: female deposits larvae on singing male crickets → larvae burrow, kill host.
- Sensory specialization
- Female auditory receptors tuned to cricket song frequency; male’s are not.
- Auditory tuning curves demonstrate sex-specific sensitivity; example of neural filtering.
Synthesis: Genetics, Development & Neural Mechanisms (Exit Activity Cue)
- Genetic architecture (e.g., supergenes) can hard-wire neural circuits or alter neurotransmitter expression → modifies behavioral phenotypes.
- Developmental plasticity influences neural connectivity (e.g., predator-induced morphs may entail neuroendocrine changes influencing behavior).
- Evolutionary perspective: natural & sexual selection shape both genes and stimulus-filtering networks – maintaining adaptive match to ecological challenges.
Key Terms & Concepts
- Phenotypic plasticity, Polyphenism, Kairomone
- Behavioral polymorphism, Supergene
- Neuron structure: dendrite, axon, synapse, action potential
- Innate behavior, Fixed Action Pattern, Releaser, Innate Releasing Mechanism
- Stimulus filtering, Cortical magnification
- Circadian clock, Sun compass, Polarized light, UV receptors
- A1/A2 receptor cells, Sonar jamming
- Predator-induced morph frequencies: \frac{18}{59}, \frac{11}{43}, \frac{30}{100}
- Monarch flight misalignment when clock shifted +6\,\text{h}
- Star-nosed mole reaction time <0.4\,\text{s}
- Sun position compensation requires continuous phase information: \text{Heading} = f(\text{Sun azimuth},\,\text{Circadian phase})
Practical & Ethical Implications
- Understanding exploitation of instincts informs conservation (e.g., brood parasite management).
- Biomimicry: Ormia-inspired directional microphones for hearing aids.
- Light pollution could disrupt monarch navigation via altered polarization patterns.
- Pesticides affecting noctuid moth hearing may cascade to bat–prey dynamics.
Suggested Study Strategies
- Draw neural pathway diagrams for moth bat-avoidance & monarch navigation circuits.
- Create flashcards for key definitions & researchers (Tinbergen, Lorenz, Catania).
- Relate sensory modalities to ecological niches: Why acoustic in nocturnal aerial hunters, tactile in subterranean foragers?
- Practice explaining how supergenes can convert plastic variation into fixed polymorphisms.