Orca behavioural indicators for marine protected area (MPA) selection.
Numbers of whale-watching vessels approximate the population size of Vancouver Island orcas.
Individuals were most vulnerable to disturbance while feeding.
Led to a focus on protecting feeding grounds from boats.
Ashe et al. 2010 Anim. Cons. 13: 196-203
Orca Behavioural Typology to Define Feeding Grounds
Observations and corresponding behaviours:
Rest: Slow with predictable coordinated short dives.
Travel: Consistent group heading with long independent dives.
Social: Tight-knit groups with irregular dives and breaching etc.
Feed: Group spread out, no consistent heading, long independent dives.
Tinbergen's Framework
Proximate Level (Individual Level):
Cause (Drivers)
Mechanism
Ontogeny (Development)
Ultimate Level (Evolutionary Level):
Function
Origin
Phylogeny
Constraints
Predictive Tools
Tinbergen’s Framework Explained
Mechanism (Causation): Immediate triggers like hormones, neural circuits, and sensory inputs.
Development (Ontogeny): How behaviour develops over an animal’s lifetime; influence of genetics and experience.
Function (Adaptive value): Survival or reproductive advantage of the behaviour; how it increases fitness.
Evolution (Phylogeny): How the behaviour evolved over time in the species’ lineage; evolutionary history.
Tinbergen’s Framework in a Conservation Context – Cetacean By-Catch
Buchholz, R. (2007) Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 22, 401-407.
Behavioural Mechanisms
Songbirds and traffic:
Generally reduced abundance near roads.
Reasons: collision, pollution, less food, visual disturbance, noise.
Studies:
Reijnen, R. & Foppen, R. (1994) J. of Applied Ecol., 31, 85-94.
Rheindt, F.E. (2003) J. für Ornithologie, 144, 295-306.
Halfwerk et al. (2011) J. Applied Ecol., 48, 210-219
Illustration of differences in song frequency of birds.
Mechanisms: Captive Breeding
Sexual selection and captive breeding of giant pandas:
Notoriously difficult to breed.
Traditionally assumed males were the choosy sex.
Behavioural observation indicated mating occurs only where both sides of a pair show high levels of courting.
Peng et al. 2009. Belg. J Zoology 139: 87-92
Behavioural Ontogeny
Captive rearing and release of whooping cranes:
Down to around 20-30 individuals in the 1940s.
Protection introduced but slow recovery.
New population deemed necessary, leading to reintroduction program.
Behavioural Ontogeny: Attempts
First attempt (1970s):
Cross-fostered with sandhill cranes.
Migration established but no breeding.
Second attempt:
Hand-rearing.
Migration not established; population not viable.
Third attempt:
Hand-rearing with puppets.
Predator aversion training.
Trained to follow microlite to establish migration.
Successful population establishment due to:
Avoiding inappropriate imprinting
Learning on non-innate behaviours
Maintenance of social structure
Function - Behavioural Science as a Conservation Tool
Interest areas don't apparently overlap, but are linked in principle: Genes Individuals Populations Species Communities
Ultimate Function (Behavioural Ecology)
Understanding behaviour from an evolutionary perspective:
Key principle: individuals behave so as to maximise their own fitness, not for the benefit of the population or species.
e.g., swallow’s tails resulting from sexual selection – increased individual fitness but reduced overall survival.
Long-forked tail streamers preferred by females, but longer tails inhibit survival by slowing flight.
Tail trait evolved to give individual males a reproductive edge, even if it makes them (or population) more vulnerable.
Conservation-Related Examples
Kakapo sex ratio problems with supplementary feeding:
Critically endangered conservation programs (many using supplementary feeding) females producing more male offspring (potential to produce more offspring and pass on genes) more males in population reduced population growth overall.
Infanticide in brown bears:
Male bears kill cubs they don’t sire (limits competitor males passing on genes and females re-enter oestrus cycle) increase in individual reproductive success.
Loss of cubs reduces juvenile survival rate and damages population growth.
Human disturbance impacting bear movements can influence this behaviour.
Allee Effects and Behaviour
Individual fitness decreases as population density or size drops.
Being rare or small population is bad for survival.
Includes:
Difficulty finding mates
Breakdown of social behaviours
Loss of learned or cultural behaviours
Reduced mate choice
Allee Effect Mechanisms in Wild Dog Packs
Foraging – defence and efficiency
Breeding – pup care and defence
Survival – defence and food sharing
Courchamp, F. & Macdonald, D.W. (2001) Anim Cons, 4, 169-174.
Allee Effect Mechanisms in Vancouver Island Marmots
Population decline and “social meltdown”
Brashares et al (2010) J. Anim. Ecol. 79: 965-973
Phylogenetic Patterns
Can behaviour help explain variation in extinction risk between species?
Some evidence that more strongly sexually-selected species (most birds) are more prone to extinction (Doherty et al. 2003).
Obvious changes in vulnerability based on behaviours as environments change (particularly with increased human impacts).
Broader Phylogenetic Patterns: Behavioural Trait Linked to Extinction Risk
Strong sexual selection:
Yes
Birds with elaborate courtship displays (such as peacocks)
Specialized foraging or habitat use:
Yes
Koala, panda
Complex social systems:
Sometimes
African wild dogs, elephants
Migration or large home ranges:
Yes
Monarch butterflies, caribou, raptors
Low behavioural flexibility:
Yes
Many island birds, corals
Behavioural Constraints as Conservation Problems
Some behaviours are maladaptive under new or human-altered conditions (even if they evolved adaptively).
Mate-finding failure
Fixed migration patterns (populations can’t adjust under human pressures and/or climate shifts)
Anti-predator behaviours not suited to introduced predators (Australian invasives and CWR mammals)
Inflexible diets so can’t adjust to shifting habitats
Protected areas aren’t always designated around animal behaviours
Integrating Behaviour into Conservation Planning
Using behavioural ecology is key to inform protected area size and design.
Monitoring wildlife behaviour as a conservation indicator (e.g. vigilance, stress, courtship, etc.).
Managing and maintaining key behaviours (e.g. migration routes).
Cultural conservation: preserving learned traditions and social structures.
Things to Think About
Linking our understanding of animal behaviour to translocations and reintroductions.
Can we modify the environment or management strategy to support key behaviours?
Ethical considerations of influencing animal behaviours.
Should we put more effort into conserving behaviours or should species evolve to adapt to a changing world?