QO

Theory of Mind (ToM) Flashcards

Theory of Mind (ToM) in Neurotypical (NT) Development: From Past to Present

Learning Objectives

  • Topic: Theory of Mind (ToM)
  • ToM in neurotypical (NT) development: Discussing studies by Wimmer & Perner (1983), Zaichik (1991), Bloom & German (2000), Onishi & Baillargeon (2005), Southgate et al. (2007), and Kulke et al. (2017; 2018).
  • Goal: To review the study of ToM from its beginnings to the present, covering past and current research, thoughts, and understandings.

What is Theory of Mind?

  • Definition: The capacity to predict and explain other people’s behavior based on their internal states of mind (emotions, intentions, desires, beliefs, perspective).
  • Encompasses: Actions, reactions, and emotions.
  • Example: If you desire something and believe it to be in a certain place, but you don't find it there, you will likely be surprised.

Origins of the Definition of ToM

  • Premack & Woodruff (1978): Action-prediction abilities in chimpanzees.
  • Example 1: Food is hanging too high for a chimp to reach, but a stool is available.
    • The correct answer involves selecting the picture of the stool.
  • Example 2: A human actor is locked in a cage with the key outside of reach, but a stick is available to drag the key closer.
    • The correct answer involves selecting the picture of the wooden stick.

Conclusions from Premack & Woodruff (1978)

  • The animals' understanding of human needs and beliefs suggests they possess a theory of mind.

Problem with Chimpanzee Studies

  • Chimps may solve the problem without inferring human beliefs or needs, relying instead on perceptual-visual experiences without higher-order cognitive abilities like ToM.
  • Question: How do we determine if a human participant truly understands others' beliefs and needs, rather than merely finding a solution?

Researching ToM

  • Question: What is an appropriate test to assess understanding of other people’s mental states?
  • Answer: The participant must attribute a belief to another person that differs from their own.
    • The participant should not be able to predict the correct response based on their own knowledge of the truth.

Rationale Behind False Belief Tasks

  • If a child can predict what another person will do based on a mistaken or false belief, it indicates they are not simply reading the answer from the presented scenario.

False Belief Task Example: Sally-Anne Task (Wimmer & Perner, 1983)

  • Characters: Sally and Anne
  • Scenario: Sally puts a marble in her basket and leaves. Anne moves the marble to her box. Sally returns.
  • Question: Where will Sally look for her marble?
  • The correct answer is the basket (where Sally falsely believes it to be).

Videos on False Belief Tasks

Appearance-Reality Task

  • Procedure: A child is shown an object and asked what it looks like.
    • 3-year-olds might say it looks like a stone.
  • The child verifies that the object is actually soft and light (a sponge).
  • The child is then asked again what it looks like.
    • 3-year-olds might say it looks like a sponge.
  • Conclusion: 3-year-olds struggle to differentiate between appearance and reality, unable to attribute a thought about the object independently of what it really is.

Deceptive Box Task

  • Procedure: A child is shown a Smarties box and asked what is inside.
    • 3-year-olds might say Smarties.
  • The box is opened, revealing a pencil instead.
  • The child is asked what another child would think is in the box.
    • 3-year-olds might say a pencil.
  • Conclusion: 3-year-olds cannot reason about thoughts independently of reality or perception.

Publications

Developmental Pattern (Leslie, 1987, 1988)

  • Learning to represent things is tied to grasping that things can represent other things.
  • Beliefs in the mind are represented in the mind by a certain age.
  • Understanding that some things can misrepresent others follows from understanding representation.

False Photograph Task (Zaitchik, 1990)

  • Involves representation but not mental states.
  • Procedure: A child is familiarized with how a Polaroid camera works. A picture is taken of a cat on a chair. While the picture develops, the cat is moved to a bed. The child is asked where the cat is in the picture.
  • Typical Findings: 3-year-olds fail, showing the same pattern as in the Sally-Anne task.

Conclusions from False Photograph Task

  • Children develop the concept of representation between 4-5 years of age.
  • Before this age, they cannot understand that beliefs can 'go out of date' and be false but can be updated with reality.
  • The problem with false belief tasks relates to the general ability to form meta-representations.

Pretend Play (Leslie, 1987, 1988)

  • Babies under 2 years old engage in pretend play (e.g., using a banana as a telephone).
  • By 18-24 months, babies form representations independent of reality and attribute mental representations to others.

Alternative Views (Bloom & German, 2000)

  • Children understand pretence and desire by 2 years of age but struggle with beliefs.
  • Young children do understand beliefs and false beliefs; the false belief task's construction leads to mistakes.

Critical Questions and Design Issues

  • What is the difference between pretence and belief as mental states?
  • Why does it take 2 years to go from understanding pretence (2 years) to understanding belief (4 years)?
  • The debate is about whether data patterns stem from belief and pretence being different or from how tasks are administered.

Cutting-Edge Evidence: Onishi & Baillargeon (2005)

  • Show that 15-month-olds understand false beliefs using a violation of expectation (VoE) paradigm.
  • Infants use mental states (goals, perceptions, and beliefs) to explain others' behavior from a young age.

ToM Research in Infancy: Onishi & Baillargeon (2005)

  • Procedural Details: Includes true belief (TB) and false belief (FB) conditions with different colored boxes.

ToM Research in Infancy: Onishi & Baillargeon (2005) Results

  • Infants look longer when the actor knows where the object is but looks for it in the wrong box.
  • Infants look longer when the actor doesn’t know where the object is but looks for it in the correct box.
  • The color of the box (green or yellow) does not affect the results.

Contemporary Research Methods

  • Anticipatory Looking Paradigm
  • Southgate et al., 2007
    • Study presented 25-month-olds with a nonverbal false-belief test using an eye tracker to measure anticipatory looking.
    • Similar paradigm to Onishi and Baillargeon (2005): An actor witnessed a toy being hidden, and the toy was later removed while the actor was not attending to the scene.
    • Question: Where would children expect the actor to search for the ball?

Anticipatory Looking Paradigm

  • Contemporary studies in ToM.
  • Videos from Kulke et al. 2018.

Debates: Current ToM Research

  • Southgate et al., 2007
  • Senju et al., 2010
  • Surian & Geraci, 2012
  • Kulke et al., 2017, 2018
  • Studies looking at Anticipatory Looking: TD and ASC.

Next Week

  • How do ToM and language development interact?
  • Are they both central in making us uniquely human?