Intertemporal Choice Overview
- Definition: Intertemporal choices refer to decisions that have future consequences or effects that manifest over time after the choice is made.
- Examples of intertemporal choices include:
- Making large purchases on credit
- Adjustments to insurance policies
- Choices concerning diet, exercise, and smoking
- Decisions regarding trust
Decision-Making in Intertemporal Choices
- How do we choose?
- We typically choose the alternative that offers the highest value or utility.
- This involves discounting delayed outcomes compared to immediate ones.
- Economic Theories:
- Neoclassical Economics/Expected Utility Theory (EUT) dictates that discounting is typically exponential.
- Exponential discounting: Involves discounting with a fixed percentage (δ) at each time step.
- Individuals may exhibit different preferences leading to variability in patience levels.
- Example in Military Pensions:
- Choice between a lump-sum payment and an annuity showed that choosing an annuity led to savings of $1.7 billion for the government.
- Factors influencing the choice included demographic traits such as being White, female, and having a college education.
- Calculated probability of reaching old age plays a significant role in these choices.
Discounting Mechanisms
- Neoclassical Economics Model:
- Characterized by exponential discounting.
- Considers fixed preference consistency over time (e.g., cake > apple today implies cake > apple tomorrow).
- Empirical Observations:
- Individuals often do not discount exponentially; instead, they discount hyperbolically.
- Hyperbolic discounting involves a fixed percentage (δ) and another constant percentage (β) that biases towards immediate rewards.
- Present Bias:
- Defined as a disproportionate preference for smaller immediate rewards over larger delayed rewards; emphasizes that 'now' is deemed special.
Two-System Model of Decision Making
- Systems of Thought:
- System 1: Fast, automatic, uncontrolled, associative, effortless, and unconscious.
- System 2: Slow, reflective, controlled, deductive, effortful, and self-aware.
- System 1 often provides intuitive solutions that are resource-efficient and serve as inputs for System 2, which engages in analysis and control.
Intertemporal Choice and the Two Systems Model
- Parameters and Neural Processes:
- The β parameter represents the value placed on immediate rewards relative to future rewards.
- The δ parameter represents the discount rate in standard exponential formulas.
- It is suggested that the different systems (System 1 and System 2) are governed by distinct neural processes.
- The reward system structures are related to β, while δ is associated with the prefrontal cortex activities, which support cognitive control functions (McClure et al. 2004).
- Neural Region Involvement:
- Area activated during decision processes includes dmPFC, dlPFC, and IFG (inferior frontal gyrus).
Evidence on Intertemporal Decision-Making
- Contrasting Views:
- Research indicates a struggle between the impatient self (System 1) and the patient self (System 2).
- Conversely, Kable & Glimcher (2007) found no evidence supporting a division into two systems in intertemporal choice; instead, activity in areas like vmPFC tracked subjective value.
- Cognitive abilities have been associated with a decreased rate of discounting delayed rewards, indicating lateral frontal cortex activity influences decision making.
- Behavioral Patterns:
- Individuals with higher cognitive ability show greater activity in lateral frontal regions when opting for delayed rewards over immediate ones.
- Steeper discount rates are correlated with behaviors like substance abuse and impulsivity, but some studies yield conflicting outcomes regarding cognitive control deficits.
Delay of Gratification Studies
- Marshmallow Test Outcomes:
- Findings indicate that children who delayed gratification performed better later in life on measures such as SAT scores, planning ability, stress management, rational thought, concentration, and overall self-control.
- The performance gap persists into adulthood, indicating a long-lasting impact of early self-control.
- Neurophysiological Factors:
- Adults who succeeded at delaying gratification showed greater lateral PFC activity during tasks requiring behavioral control.
Summary of Intertemporal Choice
- Delay Discounting:
- Delayed outcomes are consistently valued less compared to immediate ones.
- Individual differences in discounting tendencies reflect levels of patience; steeper discounting correlates with higher impatience.
- Neural Responses:
- Valuation regions exhibit varied responses aligning with the extent of delayed reward discounting, where lateral frontal areas promote patient behaviors while the striatum influences immediate reward preferences.
- Stability of Preferences:
- Immediate versus delayed reward preferences appear consistent throughout an individual’s life span and relate to self-control and success measures.
Self-Control Dynamics
- Challenges of Self-Control:
- Participation in self-control tasks usually engages the dmPFC and dlPFC during decision-making processes regarding liked but unhealthy items.
- This emphasizes the role of cognitive control networks in regulating behavior and preferences, particularly in unhealthy choices.
Persistence in Decision-Making
- Rational Persistence:
- Persistence regarding delayed rewards is rational only when the timing of those rewards can be reasonably predicted.
- Children who did not persist during the marshmallow test may have perceived their environment as unpredictable, which is predictive of negative life outcomes.
Precommitment Strategies in Intertemporal Choice
- Example of Gym Membership:
- Precommitment devices, such as a gym membership paying a fixed monthly fee versus a pay-per-visit strategy, illustrate how individuals attempt to enforce future behaviors to combat present bias. Challenges remain regarding conversion of intention into action.