Social Influence: how other people influence our behaviour
Three key types of social influence:
Compliance: change of behaviour in response to a direct request
Example: agreeing to attend a social event because someone else asked you to
Obedience: change of behaviour in response to a directive from an authority figure
Example: changing your behaviour in response to a direct order from a police officer, parent, teacher, or school official
Conformity: change in behaviour to match the response or actions of others
Example: looking in a specific direction because you saw other people doing that
Principles of Compliance (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004): applied in advertising/sales
Reciprocity: the rule that obliges us to repay others for what we have received from them
Example: acceptance of "gift" = social obligation, such as free samples
Door-in-the-face technique: starting with a large request and then lowering it after the other party says no
Example: recruiting university students for voluntary counseling positions at a detention center
Consistency: the rule that obliges us to be consistent in our behaviour
Foot-in-the-door technique: starting with a small request and then asking for a larger one
Example: attaching a small label to a house and then asking for a large sign to be placed in front of it
Commitment: once we make a commitment, we feel pressure to follow through
Low-balling technique: having people commit to a course of action and then increasing the request
Example: asking someone to participate in an experiment and then telling them it starts at 7 in the morning
Liking: people comply more with requests made by individuals they like
Factors that influence liking: physical attractiveness, similarity, familiarity
Authority: people comply more with requests made by individuals in a position of authority
Milgram's obedience studies: participants were willing to administer electric shocks to others based on orders from an authority figure
Milgram's obedience studies: predicted results for different groups
Real-world replication of Milgram's study: the Hofling et al. (1966) hospital study
Milgram's statement on participants: people who reached the end of the board were not necessarily sadistic, but were influenced by the situation
Conformity: change in behaviour to match the response or actions of others
Reasons for conformity: informational influence and normative influence
Sherif's autokinetic effect study: participants' estimates of light movement were influenced by the estimates of others
Asch's line studies: participants conformed to the incorrect answers given by others
Variations of the study: private response, non-unanimous erroneous majority, accurate information from one confederate
People perform better when alone or in groups
Audience increased cockroaches' performance on the easy task but not on the complex task
Times for simple maze: Alone - 41 sec, Audience - 33 sec
Times for complex maze: Alone - 110 sec, Audience - 130 sec
Social facilitation = improved task performance in the presence of others
Social inhibition = decreased task performance in the presence of others
Other factors that influence social facilitation / social inhibition:
Others as distraction
Evaluation apprehension
Social loafing is a type of motivation loss that occurs when group members' work is unidentifiable
Participants made less noise when they thought others were also making noise
Latané, Williams, and Harkins (1979) study:
Participants in a group of 6 people
Blindfolded participants and had them put on headphones
Played clapping or cheering over headphones
Participants asked to clap/cheer
Results: 1/3 less noise when they thought others were also making noise
Social loafing = working less in a group than individually
Prejudice (emotional): A shared attitude or feeling towards a social outgroup and their members based on group membership
Stereotypes (cognitive): Generalized beliefs about members of groups
Discrimination (behavioral): Prejudice that translates to behavior
Prejudice is not always obvious, e.g. linguistic intergroup bias
Linguistic intergroup bias:
Tendency to use concrete, specific language for positive outgroup characteristics and negative ingroup characteristics
Tendency to use more general and abstract terms for negative outgroup characteristics and positive ingroup characteristics
Illusory correlations: Perception of a relation between two elements that does not exist or is exaggerated
Example: belief that people from New York are rude
Caused by confirmatory bias, focusing on confirmatory evidence and ignoring contradictory evidence
Illusion of out-group homogeneity: Perceiving members of the outgroup as more similar to each other than members of the ingroup
Contact hypothesis: Contact with people of the outgroup should reduce prejudice
Anxiety, self-fulfilling prophecy, power differential, and length of contact can work against the contact hypothesis
Can lead to decategorization and recategorization
Bystander effect: Bystanders are less likely to help in an emergency if there are other onlookers present
Kitty Genovese case made the bystander effect well known
Latané & Darley (1968) study:
Participants discussing problems faced by students in a high-pressure urban environment
One participant on the intercom began to choke and appeared to have an epileptic seizure
Would the participant leave the room to seek help? How long would it take?
Principles that can prevent helping:
Pluralistic ignorance: Assuming others accept a belief even if they privately reject it
Diffusion of responsibility: Diluting personal responsibility for acting by assuming someone