L1 - Measurement & Errors in Medical Research – Detailed Lecture Notes
Learning Objectives
- Understand challenges in planning medical & public-health research.
- Distinguish types of measurement error, bias, & confounding.
- Clarify the difference between \text{validity} (accuracy) and \text{reliability} (precision).
Core Epidemiological Vocabulary
- Determinants / Exposures / Treatments
• Statistical notation: independent variable X in an X!\rightarrow!Y model.
• Examples: sun exposure, use of sun-beds, helmet use. - Health-related states & events / Outcomes / Diseases
• Statistical notation: dependent variable Y.
• Can be continuous, categorical, or binary; not only yes/no. - Event vs. Disease
• Event = discrete (e.g., stroke, intracerebral haemorrhage).
• Disease = chronic/long-term (e.g., melanoma). - Confounder
• A third variable associated with both X and Y, distorting their true relationship.
Ethical & Feasibility Principles
- Every research question requests time, samples, or risk from participants → respect their contribution.
- Avoid overly vulnerable populations unless justified.
- Principle of "first, do no harm" when selecting exposures or interventions.
Building a Research Question
- Generic causal template:
• “Exposure X is associated with an increase/decrease in risk of outcome Y.” - Practical brainstorming cues:
• What is happening in the population?
• \text{Incidence? Prevalence?}
• Which exposures & outcomes appear important or modifiable? - Example:
• X = wearing a motorcycle helmet.
• Y = death from head trauma.
• Hypothesis: Helmet use ↓ risk of death.
Causation vs. Association
- Holy Grail = causal inference (“cause → effect”).
- Measured with effect sizes such as relative risk RR or odds ratio OR.
• RR = \dfrac{Incidence{exposed}}{Incidence{unexposed}} - Other factors (confounders) may blur the X!\rightarrow!Y link; must be identified & controlled.
Descriptive vs. Analytical Epidemiology
- Descriptive (hypothesis generation)
• Uses “person, place, time” framework.
• Answers: Who? Where? When? - Analytical (hypothesis testing)
• Employs formal study designs (cohort, case-control, RCT, etc.).
• Requires biostatistical input: sample size, power, adjustment for confounders.
Qualities of a Worthwhile Exposure Variable
- Influences health in a modifiable way.
- Measurable with acceptable error.
- Differentiates populations (variation exists).
- Generates testable hypotheses.
- Enables prevention/control strategies.
- Feasible financially & logistically.
Qualities of a Worthwhile Outcome Variable
- Significant impact on individuals/public health (burden, severity, costs).
- Clearly defined causal pathway or risk factors.
- Measurable with valid, reliable instruments.
- If rare, still investigable via efficient designs (e.g., case-control).
Sources of Epidemiologic Hypotheses
- Individual cases or case series.
- Descriptive data (registries, surveillance).
- Data mining (“dredging”) of large datasets.
- Laboratory/animal studies suggesting human parallels.
- Analogies from other exposure–outcome pairs.
- Common sense & everyday observations.
- Conversations with colleagues, clinicians, patients, families.
- Media (newspapers, TV) highlighting emerging issues.
Practical Search Strategy for Hypotheses
- Read primary literature & systematic reviews.
- Examine public datasets for trends/anomalies.
- Conduct stakeholder interviews (patients, clinicians).
- Stay alert to news reports of outbreaks or controversies.
Recurring Methodological Challenges
- Chronic-disease etiology often multi-factorial & time-lagged.
- One exposure can yield multiple outcomes; one outcome can stem from multiple exposures.
- Different individuals may arrive at the same disease via distinct causal pathways (gene–environment interplay, etc.).
- Observational nature of most epidemiologic work → limited control over exposure assignment.
Observational vs. Experimental Designs
- Observational studies: cohort, case-control, cross-sectional.
• Do not manipulate exposure; rely on “inadvertent” or naturally occurring exposure patterns (e.g., workplace, geography). - Experimental study (clinical trial)
• Only major exception where humans are deliberately assigned an exposure/intervention, under strict ethical oversight.
Role of Biological Plausibility
- Statistical associations strengthened when consistent with known pathophysiology.
- Requires interdisciplinary understanding of biology, pathology, toxicology.
Example Walk-Through: Skin Cancer Project
- Exposure candidates:
• Sun exposure dosage.
• Artificial tanning (sun-beds). - Outcomes:
• Melanoma incidence.
• Mortality in severe cases. - Design considerations:
• If melanoma is rare in target population, case-control design efficient.
• Need accurate exposure assessment (UV dose, tanning frequency).
• Potential confounders: skin type, sunscreen use, family history.
Example Walk-Through: Helmet Campaign
- Research underpinning public-health ad:
• Statement: “I won’t wear a helmet—it makes me look stupid.”
• Visual: injured youth, older caregiver hands feeding them → emotional resonance. - Data used: prior studies quantifying risk reduction in head trauma fatalities with helmet use.
- Ethical/communication lesson: translating epidemiologic evidence into persuasive health-promotion messages.
Measurement Error, Bias & Confounding (Preview)
- Measurement error: deviation between observed & true value (random vs. systematic).
- Bias: systematic deviation of study results from truth (selection, information, publication, etc.).
- Confounding: mixing of effects—third variable distorts X-Y relation; addressed via design (randomisation, restriction, matching) or analysis (stratification, regression).
Validity vs. Reliability
- Validity (accuracy): how close a measurement is to truth.
- Reliability (precision): how consistently the measurement can be repeated.
- Interaction: a tool can be reliable but invalid; optimal instruments aim for both.
Key Take-Home Messages
- Formulating a precise, feasible research question is foundational; respect participants’ contribution.
- Always weigh ethical, financial, and logistical constraints when selecting exposures & outcomes.
- Use descriptive epi to observe patterns, then analytical epi to test causal hypotheses.
- Collaboration with biostatisticians maximises study efficiency & validity.
- Epidemiology seeks population-level associations; individual causation usually indeterminable.
- Observational designs dominate, but well-designed trials offer the strongest causal inference within ethical bounds.