Personality Theory Flashcards (Video Transcript)
Introduction
Transcript begins with a real-world contrast: Bill Clinton and his half-brother Roger Clinton, raised by the same people, took different paths. Uses this to motivate the study of personality and the internal forces shaping decisions.
Core question: What internal forces shape our decisions? How can personality psychology help answer it?
Learning objectives introduced:
Define personality
Describe early theories about personality development
What is Personality?
Personality refers to long-standing traits and patterns that propel individuals to consistently think, feel, and behave in specific ways.
Our personality makes us unique individuals; it tends to be long-term, stable, and not easily changed.
Etymology: The word “personality” comes from the Latin word persona, meaning a mask; historically, a mask represented or projected a trait of a character.
Reflection question: What characteristics describe your own personality?
Historical Perspectives on Personality
Ancient roots: Hippocrates proposed four temperaments tied to bodily humors:
Choleric: yellow bile from the liver (passionate, ambitious, bold)
Melancholic: black bile from the kidneys (reserved, anxious, unhappy)
Sanguine: red blood from the heart (joyful, eager, optimistic)
Phlegmatic: white phlegm from the lungs (calm, reliable, thoughtful)
These ideas were later extended by Galen, who linked disease and personality to humoral imbalances.
Medieval to early modern: Galen’s four temperaments remained influential for centuries.
1780 phrenology: Franz Gall proposed that bumps on the skull reflect personality traits and underlying brain areas; used to infer languages, kindness, aggression, etc. Discredited as pseudoscience due to lack of empirical support.
Kant and Wundt:
Kant proposed trait words describing each temperament but believed categories were non-overlapping.
Wundt introduced a two-axis model: emotional vs. non-emotional and changeable vs. unchangeable.
Freud and the psychodynamic perspective (early 20th century): the first comprehensive theory of personality integrating unconscious drives and childhood experiences.
Freud's influence persisted through Neo-Freudians, who retained childhood influence but emphasized social/cultural factors over pure sexuality.
Other major perspectives emerged later: learning, humanistic, biological, evolutionary, and cultural.
Freud: Psychodynamic Perspective
Core idea: Unconscious drives, especially sex and aggression, shape personality. Childhood sexuality and experiences influence adult behavior.
Levels of consciousness (the iceberg analogy): only a small portion of our mind is conscious; the majority is unconscious and influences behavior.
Unconscious mind contains unacceptable urges; repression keeps them out of awareness.
Freudian slips (slips of the tongue) are seen as reflections of unconscious desires.
Approximate view from Freud: only about ~1/10 of the mind is conscious; the rest is unconscious. ext{Conscious}
oughly 0.1 imes ext{total mind}
Structural model: id, ego, superego
Id: primitive drives, present from birth; operates on the pleasure principle (immediate gratification).
Ego: the rational self that balances id and superego within reality; operates on the reality principle.
Superego: internalized societal rules and morality; acts as conscience.
Dynamic: ego balances id’s desires with superego’s moral constraints; when imbalance occurs, anxiety arises.
Neurotic outcomes: an imbalance among id, ego, and superego can lead to neurosis (negative emotions, anxiety, unhealthy behaviors).
Defense mechanisms: unconscious tactics the ego uses to reduce anxiety and protect the psyche; examples include repression, reaction formation, regression, projection, rationalization, displacement, sublimation.
Example scenario: a student (Joe) sublimates sexual tension by acting macho, making derogatory jokes about others; this is a form of defense mechanism use.
Defense mechanism examples:
Repression: pushing distressing memories from consciousness.
Reaction formation: expressing opposite feelings to unconscious impulses.
Regression: acting younger to cope with stress.
Projection: attributing own unacceptable feelings to others.
Rationalization, displacement, sublimation: other ways the mind distorts or diverts urges.
Psychosexual stages of development:
Oral (0-1 year): pleasure focused on the mouth; fixation can lead to oral traits (smoking, overeating, thumb-sucking).
Anal (1-3 years): toilet training; fixations can lead to anal-retentive (order, stinginess) or anal-expulsive (messiness) traits.
Phallic (3-6 years): genitals; Oedipus complex in boys; Electra complex in girls (originally proposed by Jung; Freud later revised stance). Castration anxiety in boys; identification with father.
Latency: sexual feelings dormant; focus on school, friendships, hobbies; consolidation of gender roles.
Genital: puberty onward; mature sexual interests; fixation unresolved can lead to issues in adulthood.
Controversies and developments:
Freud’s theories are controversial and not fully supported by modern research; however, he highlighted the importance of early childhood experiences and unconscious processes.
Later psychoanalysts expanded on social/cultural aspects (Neo-Freudians) and broadened emphasis beyond sexuality.
Electra complex: proposed by Carl Jung and discussed in the context of penis envy; Freud’s stance evolved, but the concept remains influential in some discussions.
Other Notable Early Theorists (Post-Freud)
Alfred Adler (Individual Psychology):
Break from Freud; focused on social motives and conscious rather than sexual motives.
Inferiority complex: deep feelings of worthlessness driving striving for superiority.
Emphasized social connections and three fundamental social tasks: occupational, societal/friendship, and love/romance.
Birth order effects: older siblings may become overachievers; youngest may be spoiled; middle child dynamics may influence personality. Evidence for these claims is mixed; research is inconclusive.
Erik Erikson (Psychosocial Theory):
Emphasized lifespan development; eight stages, each with a central conflict to resolve for healthy personality development.
Carl Jung (Analytical Psychology):
Split from Freud on two major points: sexuality is not the sole driver; adds the concept of the collective unconscious and archetypes.
Collective unconscious: universal, inherited patterns (archetypes) shared across cultures (e.g., hero, maiden, sage, trickster).
Extroversion vs. introversion: energy source differences; balance between them considered essential for self-realization.
Persona: the mask we present to the world; a compromise between our true self and social expectations.
Archetypes are potentially universal but modern research questions a strictly biological basis for archetypes; may emerge from experiences and culture.
Karen Horney (Psychoanalytic/cultural critique):
Rejected Freud’s penis envy as a universal female trait; instead argued social/cultural factors explain gender differences.
Emphasized basic anxiety from unmet needs and proposed three neurotic coping styles: moving toward people (need for affection), moving against people (aggression, dominance), moving away from people (detachment).
These coping styles can become rigid and neurotic if overused.
The Learning Perspectives on Personality
Shift from internal drives to observable behavior and environmental reinforcements.
Behavioral perspective (Skinner): personality is shaped by environment; stable patterns arise from learned response tendencies; personality can change across the lifespan with changes in reinforcement structures.
Example: Greta’s speeding behavior declines after marriage and children due to changed reinforcement/punishment contexts.
Social Cognitive Perspective (Bandura): adds cognition to learning; emphasizes reciprocal determinism, observational learning, and self-efficacy.
Reciprocal determinism: behavior, cognitive processes, and environment all influence each other.
Observational learning: learning by watching models and their consequences; imitation depends on whether the model is reinforced or punished.
Self-efficacy: belief in one’s own abilities to achieve goals; high self-efficacy leads to more persistence and resilience; low self-efficacy can limit challenge engagement.
Example: someone with high self-efficacy may tackle a challenging task with confidence; someone with low self-efficacy may avoid it.
Locus of Control (Rotter): internal vs. external control of outcomes.
Internal locus: outcomes result from one’s own efforts; associated with better academic performance, independence, and health.
External locus: outcomes result from luck, fate, or other people; associated with poorer adjustment in some domains.
Walter Mischel and the Person-Situation Debate: concluded that behavior varies across situations more than per trait alone, though consistency exists within similar situations and across time in equivalent contexts.
Marshmallow test (Self-regulation): delay of gratification predicts later life outcomes; higher self-control associated with better academic achievement, healthier relationships, and fewer problems; later research recognizes situational factors moderate these associations.
Concept of self-regulation (willpower): goal-directed control using internal/external feedback to maximize goal attainment.
Conclusion: both situational factors and personal dispositions shape behavior; humans are situation processors.
The Humanistic Perspective
Third force in psychology, reacting to psychoanalytic and behaviorist extremes.
Emphasizes growth, self-actualization, and subjective experience.
Abraham Maslow:
Hierarchy of needs: from basic physiological needs to safety, love/belonging, esteem, and self-actualization at the top.
Self-actualization: realizing one’s fullest potential; people who reach this level tend to be open, creative, loving, spontaneous, compassionate, self-accepting.
Carl Rogers:
Self-concept: thoughts and feelings about oneself; the real self vs. ideal self.
Congruence: similarity between real self and ideal self; high congruence linked to higher self-worth and healthier functioning.
Unconditional positive regard: acceptance that facilitates growth; lack of congruence can lead to maladjustment.
Your answers to “Who am I?” reflect self-concept; congruence fosters healthy development.
Biological and Evolutionary Perspectives on Personality
Genetic and physiological bases for personality:
Minnesota Twin Study (Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory context): identical twins raised apart or together show substantial similarity in some traits; suggests heritability for certain traits.
Heritability (h^2): proportion of observed variation in a population attributable to genetics; traits showing h^2 > 0.5 indicate strong genetic influence for those traits.
Important caveat: traits are polygenic (involve many genes) and also shaped by epigenetic factors and environment.
Temperament as a biologically-based set of tendencies apparent early in life; Thomas and Chess identified three main temperaments: easy, difficult, slow-to-warm-up.
Modern view emphasizes two key temperament dimensions: reactivity and self-regulation; environmental factors influence expression.
Evolutionary perspective:
Life history theory: how people allocate time and energy to growth, reproduction, and parenting.
Costly signaling theory: how traits may signal mate quality or social status honestly or deceptively.
Cross-cultural and genetic variation:
Twin studies, cross-cultural research show both universal patterns and cultural differences in personality traits.
Trait Theories of Personality
Trait theorists describe personality in terms of relatively stable traits present across people.
Allport (1930s): trait taxonomy with three levels:
Cardinal traits: dominant traits that shape entire personality (rare).
Central (central) traits: basic building blocks of personality (e.g., loyalty, kindness).
Secondary traits: less obvious, context-specific preferences and attitudes.
Raymond Cattell (1957): reduced trait list to 16 factors (16PF):
Dimensions include warmth, reasoning, emotional stability, dominance, liveliness, rule-consciousness, social boldness, sensitivity, vigilance, abstractedness, privateness, apprehension, openness to change, self-reliance, tension.
Assessment uses a continuum for each trait (not simply present/absent).
Hans and Sybil Eysenck: two-factor model (later three-factor)
Core dimensions: Extraversion–Introversion; Neuroticism–Stability.
Later added a third dimension: Psychoticism–Superego control (impulse control vs. anti-social tendencies).
Eysenck’s model has been critiqued for being too narrow; nonetheless influential.
Big Five (Five Factor Model): the dominant contemporary framework
Five core factors: Openness to Experience (O), Conscientiousness (C), Extraversion (E), Agreeableness (A), Neuroticism (N).
Mnemonic: OCEAN or CANOE (O, C, E, A, N).
Each trait exists on a continuum; most people score somewhere in the middle.
Cross-cultural stability: traits exist across cultures and ethnicities; likely have biological and genetic components.
Longevity of traits: some changes across lifespan; conscientiousness and agreeableness tend to increase with age, while neuroticism and sometimes extraversion decline slightly with age.
HEXACO model (an alternative model): Honesty-Humility, Emotionality, Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Openness to experience.
Cross-cultural considerations:
Individualist cultures (e.g., US, Western Europe) emphasize independence and personal achievement; collectivist cultures emphasize social harmony and group needs.
Culture can influence how traits manifest and how tests are interpreted; some inventories may require culturally specific adaptations.
Regional personality patterns in the US identified through large samples (Rentfro et al., 2013): three clusters with different profiles (Upper Midwest/Deep South; West; Northeast) and associations with openness/conscientiousness, stress, and political leanings.
Cross-cultural approaches to studying personality:
Cultural comparative approach: tests Western theories in other cultures; aims for generalizability.
Indigenous approach: develop culture-specific measures that reflect local constructs.
Combined approach: integrates Western and indigenous methods to capture universal and culture-specific traits.
Culture and Personality
Culture shapes personality through language, socialization, and modeling of culturally appropriate behaviors.
Universal vs. culture-specific dimensions:
Some traits show cross-cultural validity (Big Five broadly observed), but the expression and importance of traits can vary by culture.
Individualist vs. collectivist cultures influence the emphasis on personal vs. group-related traits.
Examples:
Yang (2006) found more personally oriented traits in individualist cultures and more socially oriented traits in collectivist cultures.
Cross-cultural test validity issues: tests like Eysenck’s inventory may function differently in collectivist settings; test translation and cultural context matter.
Cross-cultural testing in minority populations: culturally relevant tests (e.g., Native American contexts) require careful interpretation and culture-specific norms.
Approaches to Studying Personality in a Cultural Context
Three main approaches:
Cultural comparative approach: tests Western theories in other cultures to examine universality.
Indigenous approach: builds instruments grounded in the culture’s own constructs.
Combined approach: uses both Western and indigenous insights to understand universal and cultural variations.
Personality Assessment: Methods and Applications
Self-report inventories (objective tests):
Usually use Likert-type scales (one to five) and multiple-choice items.
Pros: easy to administer, cost-effective.
Cons: susceptible to social desirability bias and intentional distortion.
Example usage: employment screening, clinical assessment, counseling.
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI): a widely used clinical inventory
First published in 1943; MMPI-2 updated in 1989.
MMPI-2-RF updated version (2008) with 338 items; shorter and more efficient.
Administration: largely computer-based in modern practice; takes about 1–2 hours for MMPI-2; 20–35 minutes for MMPI-2-RF.
Scales in MMPI-2: a clinical profile consists of 10 main scales: Hypochondriasis (Hs), Depression (D), Hysteria (Hy), Psychopathic Deviate (Pd), Social Introversion (Si), Masculinity-Femininity (Mf), Paranoia (Pa), Psychasthenia (Pt), Schizophrenia (Sc), Hypomania (Ma).
MMPI-2 also includes an alcoholism risk scale (or related validity indicators) and validity scales such as the Lie scale (L) to detect faking good/bad and other response biases.
MMPI-2RF feature: three validity/interpretive scales with updated structure for reliability and efficiency.
Uses: clinical diagnosis, occupational screening (e.g., law enforcement), counseling, custody evaluations.
Reliability and validity concepts apply: test-retest reliability, construct validity, criterion validity, and the risk of bias with self-reports.
Projective tests (strategies to access unconscious processes):
Based on defense mechanisms and the assumption that ambiguous stimuli elicit projection of internal states.
Rorschach Inkblot Test (1921): clients describe what they see in a series of inkblots; standardized with Exner scoring system to assess depression, psychosis, anxiety, etc.
Thematic Apperception Test (TAT, 1930s): clients tell stories about ambiguous pictures; aims to reveal social world, hopes, fears, goals; storytelling reduces resistance to disclosure; standardization is weak and validity/reliability vary.
Rotter Incomplete Sentence Blank (RISB, 1950): 40 incomplete sentences; quick completions reveal personal concerns and adjustment issues; used for college/career counseling.
Contemporized Theme Concerning Black’s Test (CTCB, Williams, 1972): culturally relevant adaptation for African Americans; increases story length and identification.
The TEMIS (Tell Me a Story): culturally tailored to minority groups (e.g., Hispanic youths).
Issues: traditional projective tests have biases in cross-cultural contexts; standardization challenges; modern practice includes more culturally sensitive alternatives.
Cross-cultural considerations in testing:
Test bias and cultural bias have limited usefulness in diverse populations when using tests developed in one culture.
Culturally relevant test development (CTCB, TEMIS) improves interpretive validity for minority groups.
Summary and Practical Implications
Personality theories provide frameworks to understand why people differ in thought, feeling, and behavior.
Classic theories (Freud, Jung, Adler, Horney) offer insights into early development, unconscious processes, archetypes, and social factors.
Learning and cognitive approaches emphasize how environment, reinforcement, observational learning, and self-efficacy shape behavior and personality across the lifespan.
Humanistic theories emphasize self-actualization, congruence, and unconditional positive regard as routes to healthy personality.
Biological and evolutionary perspectives highlight genetic influences and adaptive advantages of certain traits, while temperament reflects early biologically-based tendencies.
Trait theories (Big Five, HEXACO) provide stable dimensions to describe personality across cultures, with evidence for universal patterns but culture-specific expressions.
Cross-cultural research underscores the role of culture in shaping personality and cautions against universal generalizations; culturally grounded assessment improves validity.
Personality assessment blends self-reports and projective methods, each with strengths and limitations; practical use in clinical, organizational, and forensic settings requires awareness of biases and culture-specific factors.
Key Formulas and Numerical References (LaTeX)
Freudian iceberg approximation (levels of consciousness): approximately ext{conscious mind}
ightarrow rac{1}{10} ext{ of total mind} and the rest is unconscious.Big Five factors (OCEAN) as a model: O= ext{Openness},\nC= ext{Conscientiousness},
E= ext{Extraversion},
A= ext{Agreeableness},
N= ext{Neuroticism}. A mnemonic: ext{OCEAN}.Locus of control continuum: internal vs external control; no single numeric formula, but conceptually with outcomes linked to personal effort vs. luck.
Heritability concept (example): if a trait shows heritability h^2 > 0.5, genetics account for more than half of the observed variation in that trait within a population.
Minnesota Twin Study (conceptual): identical twins raised apart show substantial similarity on several traits, implying genetic influence; exact numeric values vary by trait.