Physio-Bio Psych | Unit IV to VI | Final Exam Reviewer
Lesson 10. Learning and Memory
Localization of Memory
There are three basic approaches.
1. Imaging. Modern imaging techniques like fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) or
PET (positron emission tomography) allows one to “see” areas of the brain that are active during
specific brain tasks. If a subject is placed in an fMRI scanner and given a memory test, one can
determine what areas of the brain are active, and that activity presumably is related to where in
the brain the memory is processed and/or stored.
The figure above illustrates an example of a PET scan of an individual who is performing
an object location test. The color code is such that the brighter, redder regions indicate increased
brain activity. The most active region is the hippocampus. In discussions of memory, the
hippocampus is mentioned repeatedly because it is a major part of the brain involved in
declarative memory function. This illustration clearly indicates that the hippocampus is involved
in object location memory. But as we will see soon, it is not where all memories are stored.
2. Brain lesions. In this experimental procedure, small parts of the brains of mice or rats are
surgically removed or chemically inactivated and the animals are systematically examined to
determine whether the lesion affected any memory system.
3. Brain disease and injury. Here scientists take advantage of individuals who have had
unfortunate brain injuries, for example, through stroke or through a brain tumor in a specific area
of the brain. If one finds a memory deficit in the patient, it is likely that the region of the brain that
was injured is involved in that memory.
The figure below summarizes many decades of research on the anatomical locus of
memory systems. The medial temporal lobe and structures like the hippocampus are involved
with memories for facts and events; the striatum is involved with memories for skills and habits;
the neocortex is involved with priming; the amygdala is involved with emotional memories; and
the cerebellum with simple forms of associative learning. Lower brain regions and the spinal cord
contain even simpler forms of learning. In summary, memory is not stored in a single place in the
brain. It is distributed in different parts of the brain.
Lesson 11. Types of Memory
Psychologists and neuroscientists have divided memory systems into two broad
categories, declarative and non-declarative. The declarative memory system is the system of
memory that is perhaps the most familiar. It is the memory system that has a conscious
component and it includes the memories of facts and events. A fact like 'Paris is the capital of
France', or an event like a prior vacation to Paris. Non-declarative memory, also called implicit
memory, includes the types of memory systems that do not have a conscious component but are
nevertheless extremely important. They include the memories for skills and habits (e.g., riding a
bicycle, driving a car, playing golf or tennis or a piano), a phenomenon called priming, simple
forms of associative learning [e.g., classical conditioning (Pavlovian conditioning)], and finally
simple forms of non-associative learning such as habituation and sensitization. Declarative
memory is "knowing what" and non-declarative memory is "knowing how".
Everyone is interested in knowing how well they remember so let us take a simple memory
test. This test will present a list of 15 words (see photo below), then there will be a pause and
you will be asked whether you remember some of those words. You have to put your pen down
for this test.
This memory test called the DRM test after its creators James Deese, Henry Roediger
and Kathleen McDermott. It was not meant to be a trick, but to illustrate a very interesting and
important feature about memory. We like to think that memory is similar to taking a photograph
and placing that photograph into a filing cabinet drawer to be withdrawn later (recalled) as the
“memory” exactly the way it was placed there originally (stored). But memory is more like taking
a picture and tearing it up into small pieces and putting the pieces in different drawers. The
memory is then recalled by reconstructing the memory from the individual fragments of the
memory. The reason so many individuals incorrectly believe that “sweet” was on the list is
because there were so many other words on the list that had a sweet connotation. “Failing” this
test is actually not a bad outcome. Individuals with Alzheimer’s disease generally do not say that
“sweet” was on the list. They cannot make the normal associations involved in the recall of a
memory.
The word list gives insights into memory processing and retrieval, but it is not a really good
test of “raw” memory ability because it can be affected by distortions and biases. To avoid these
problems, psychologists have developed other memory tests. One is the object recognition test
to test declarative memory. This test is also good because it can even be used on animals. The
test involves presenting a subject with two different objects and they are asked to remember those
objects. There is a pause and then two objects are shown again, one of which is new and the
other having been shown previously. Subjects are asked to identify the novel object, and to do
so, they need to remember which one was shown previously. A somewhat related test is the
object location test in which subjects are asked to remember the location of an object on a two-
dimensional surface.
Examples of non-declarative memory, such as associative learning, can be tested by
pairing one stimulus with another and later testing whether a subject has learned to make the
association between the two stimuli. The classical example is the paradigm developed by the
Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov, which is now called classical or Pavlovian conditioning. In
classical conditioning, a novel or weak stimulus (conditioned stimulus, CS) like a sound is paired
with a stimulus like food that generally elicits a reflexive response (unconditioned response, UR;
unconditioned stimulus, US) such as salivation. After sufficient training with contingent CS-US
presentations (which may be a single trial), the CS is capable of eliciting a response (conditioned
response, CR), which often resembles the UR (or some aspect of it).
Lesson 12. Types of Amnesia
A classic study on localization of memory was the result of surgery performed on Henry
Molaison, a patient who was only known to the scientific community as “H.M.” until his death in
2008. H. M. is famous in neuroscience literature because his brain provided major insights into
the localization of memory function. In the 1950’s, H.M. was diagnosed with intractable epilepsy,
and while there are pharmacologic treatments, in some cases the only treatment is to remove the
portion of the brain that is causing the seizures. Consequently, H.M.'s hippocampus was removed
bilaterally. The figure on the right is an MRI of a normal individual showing the hippocampal
region, whereas the figure on the left shows a MRI of patient H.M. after the removal of the
hippocampus.
Before the operation, H.M. had a fine memory, but after the operation, H.M. had a very
severe memory deficit. Specifically, after the operation H.M.'s ability to form any new memories
for facts and events was severely impaired; he had great difficulty learning any new vocabulary
words; he could not remember what happened the day before. So if H.M. had an interview the
day following a previous interview, he would have little or no memory about the interview or events
during it. This study clearly indicated that the hippocampus was critical for memory formation. But
whereas H.M. had great difficulty forming new memories for facts and events, he still had all of
his old memories for facts and events. Specifically, he had all his childhood memories, and all of
his memories prior to the operation. This type of memory deficit is called anterograde amnesia.
(In contrast, retrograde amnesia refers to loss of old memories.) The studies on H.M. clearly
indicated that whereas the hippocampus is critical for the formation of new memories, it is not
where the old memories are stored. It is now known that those old memories are stored in other
parts of the brain, such as in the frontal cortex. The process by which an initially labile memory is
transformed into a more enduring form is called consolidation. This process involves the memory
being stored in a different part of the brain than the initial site of its encoding.
H.M. was also interesting in that while his ability to form new memories for facts and events
was severely impaired, he could form new memories for skills and habits. While he could form
new memories for skills and habits, he did not know that he had the skills! He had no awareness
of the memory; he couldn’t declare that he had it. This finding clearly indicated that the memory
for skills and habits are not formed in the hippocampus. Collectively, we learned from these
studies on H.M. and other patients that memory is distributed throughout the nervous system, and
different brain regions are involved in mediating different types of memory.
Lesson 10. Learning and Memory
Localization of Memory
There are three basic approaches.
1. Imaging. Modern imaging techniques like fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) or
PET (positron emission tomography) allows one to “see” areas of the brain that are active during
specific brain tasks. If a subject is placed in an fMRI scanner and given a memory test, one can
determine what areas of the brain are active, and that activity presumably is related to where in
the brain the memory is processed and/or stored.
The figure above illustrates an example of a PET scan of an individual who is performing
an object location test. The color code is such that the brighter, redder regions indicate increased
brain activity. The most active region is the hippocampus. In discussions of memory, the
hippocampus is mentioned repeatedly because it is a major part of the brain involved in
declarative memory function. This illustration clearly indicates that the hippocampus is involved
in object location memory. But as we will see soon, it is not where all memories are stored.
2. Brain lesions. In this experimental procedure, small parts of the brains of mice or rats are
surgically removed or chemically inactivated and the animals are systematically examined to
determine whether the lesion affected any memory system.
3. Brain disease and injury. Here scientists take advantage of individuals who have had
unfortunate brain injuries, for example, through stroke or through a brain tumor in a specific area
of the brain. If one finds a memory deficit in the patient, it is likely that the region of the brain that
was injured is involved in that memory.
The figure below summarizes many decades of research on the anatomical locus of
memory systems. The medial temporal lobe and structures like the hippocampus are involved
with memories for facts and events; the striatum is involved with memories for skills and habits;
the neocortex is involved with priming; the amygdala is involved with emotional memories; and
the cerebellum with simple forms of associative learning. Lower brain regions and the spinal cord
contain even simpler forms of learning. In summary, memory is not stored in a single place in the
brain. It is distributed in different parts of the brain.
Lesson 11. Types of Memory
Psychologists and neuroscientists have divided memory systems into two broad
categories, declarative and non-declarative. The declarative memory system is the system of
memory that is perhaps the most familiar. It is the memory system that has a conscious
component and it includes the memories of facts and events. A fact like 'Paris is the capital of
France', or an event like a prior vacation to Paris. Non-declarative memory, also called implicit
memory, includes the types of memory systems that do not have a conscious component but are
nevertheless extremely important. They include the memories for skills and habits (e.g., riding a
bicycle, driving a car, playing golf or tennis or a piano), a phenomenon called priming, simple
forms of associative learning [e.g., classical conditioning (Pavlovian conditioning)], and finally
simple forms of non-associative learning such as habituation and sensitization. Declarative
memory is "knowing what" and non-declarative memory is "knowing how".
Everyone is interested in knowing how well they remember so let us take a simple memory
test. This test will present a list of 15 words (see photo below), then there will be a pause and
you will be asked whether you remember some of those words. You have to put your pen down
for this test.
This memory test called the DRM test after its creators James Deese, Henry Roediger
and Kathleen McDermott. It was not meant to be a trick, but to illustrate a very interesting and
important feature about memory. We like to think that memory is similar to taking a photograph
and placing that photograph into a filing cabinet drawer to be withdrawn later (recalled) as the
“memory” exactly the way it was placed there originally (stored). But memory is more like taking
a picture and tearing it up into small pieces and putting the pieces in different drawers. The
memory is then recalled by reconstructing the memory from the individual fragments of the
memory. The reason so many individuals incorrectly believe that “sweet” was on the list is
because there were so many other words on the list that had a sweet connotation. “Failing” this
test is actually not a bad outcome. Individuals with Alzheimer’s disease generally do not say that
“sweet” was on the list. They cannot make the normal associations involved in the recall of a
memory.
The word list gives insights into memory processing and retrieval, but it is not a really good
test of “raw” memory ability because it can be affected by distortions and biases. To avoid these
problems, psychologists have developed other memory tests. One is the object recognition test
to test declarative memory. This test is also good because it can even be used on animals. The
test involves presenting a subject with two different objects and they are asked to remember those
objects. There is a pause and then two objects are shown again, one of which is new and the
other having been shown previously. Subjects are asked to identify the novel object, and to do
so, they need to remember which one was shown previously. A somewhat related test is the
object location test in which subjects are asked to remember the location of an object on a two-
dimensional surface.
Examples of non-declarative memory, such as associative learning, can be tested by
pairing one stimulus with another and later testing whether a subject has learned to make the
association between the two stimuli. The classical example is the paradigm developed by the
Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov, which is now called classical or Pavlovian conditioning. In
classical conditioning, a novel or weak stimulus (conditioned stimulus, CS) like a sound is paired
with a stimulus like food that generally elicits a reflexive response (unconditioned response, UR;
unconditioned stimulus, US) such as salivation. After sufficient training with contingent CS-US
presentations (which may be a single trial), the CS is capable of eliciting a response (conditioned
response, CR), which often resembles the UR (or some aspect of it).
Lesson 12. Types of Amnesia
A classic study on localization of memory was the result of surgery performed on Henry
Molaison, a patient who was only known to the scientific community as “H.M.” until his death in
2008. H. M. is famous in neuroscience literature because his brain provided major insights into
the localization of memory function. In the 1950’s, H.M. was diagnosed with intractable epilepsy,
and while there are pharmacologic treatments, in some cases the only treatment is to remove the
portion of the brain that is causing the seizures. Consequently, H.M.'s hippocampus was removed
bilaterally. The figure on the right is an MRI of a normal individual showing the hippocampal
region, whereas the figure on the left shows a MRI of patient H.M. after the removal of the
hippocampus.
Before the operation, H.M. had a fine memory, but after the operation, H.M. had a very
severe memory deficit. Specifically, after the operation H.M.'s ability to form any new memories
for facts and events was severely impaired; he had great difficulty learning any new vocabulary
words; he could not remember what happened the day before. So if H.M. had an interview the
day following a previous interview, he would have little or no memory about the interview or events
during it. This study clearly indicated that the hippocampus was critical for memory formation. But
whereas H.M. had great difficulty forming new memories for facts and events, he still had all of
his old memories for facts and events. Specifically, he had all his childhood memories, and all of
his memories prior to the operation. This type of memory deficit is called anterograde amnesia.
(In contrast, retrograde amnesia refers to loss of old memories.) The studies on H.M. clearly
indicated that whereas the hippocampus is critical for the formation of new memories, it is not
where the old memories are stored. It is now known that those old memories are stored in other
parts of the brain, such as in the frontal cortex. The process by which an initially labile memory is
transformed into a more enduring form is called consolidation. This process involves the memory
being stored in a different part of the brain than the initial site of its encoding.
H.M. was also interesting in that while his ability to form new memories for facts and events
was severely impaired, he could form new memories for skills and habits. While he could form
new memories for skills and habits, he did not know that he had the skills! He had no awareness
of the memory; he couldn’t declare that he had it. This finding clearly indicated that the memory
for skills and habits are not formed in the hippocampus. Collectively, we learned from these
studies on H.M. and other patients that memory is distributed throughout the nervous system, and
different brain regions are involved in mediating different types of memory.