Evidence: Infants can discriminate phonemes from birth.
High-Amplitude Sucking (HAS) Technique:
Measures changes in an infant’s sucking rate in response to auditory stimuli; an increased sucking rate indicates perception of change.
Study Example: Eimas et al. (1971) showed that one-month-old infants could differentiate between /ba/ and /pa/ based on voice onset time (VOT).
Prenatal Studies:
Fetal heart rate deceleration and MEG studies show fetuses (around 28 weeks gestation) can detect changes in auditory stimuli, especially prosodic features.
Native Language Preference: Newborns prefer their native language, suggesting phonological learning begins before birth.
Theoretical Alignment:
Supports nativist theories (Chomsky, Pinker).
Integrates early learning from environmental exposure.
Segmentation Problem: Infants must identify individual words within a continuous stream of spoken language without explicit boundaries.
Strategies:
Statistical Learning:
Saffran et al. (1996) showed infants track syllable co-occurrence probabilities.
High transitional probabilities indicate syllables belong to the same word; low probabilities indicate word boundaries.
Phonotactic Constraints:
Infants become sensitive to permissible sound sequences in their language.
Example: English-speaking infants learn that "ng" is unlikely at the beginning of words.
Prosodic Bootstrapping:
Infants use rhythm, stress, and intonation to infer word boundaries.
English two-syllable words often have a trochaic stress pattern (strong-weak).
By 7.5 months, infants use this pattern to segment speech, preferentially extracting words beginning with a stressed syllable.
Familiar Words as Anchors:
Learned words serve as reference points to segment adjacent speech.
Age: Begins around 10-12 months.
Fast Mapping:
Children form a connection between a new word and its referent after minimal exposure.
Carey and Bartlett (1978) demonstrated that children could remember a novel word's meaning after hearing it once in a clear context.
Innate Biases and Heuristics:
Mutual Exclusivity: Children assume each object has only one label, ruling out known terms when hearing a new word.
Syntactic Bootstrapping: Inferring word meaning based on grammatical structure.
Pragmatic Cues: Using gaze direction and speaker intention.
Vocabulary Development:
Non-linear trajectory with U-shaped learning patterns.
Initially using correct forms, then overgeneralizing rules (e.g., “goed” instead of “went”), and eventually returning to correct usage after learning exceptions.
Cognitive Mechanisms: Combining domain-specific tools (attention to language) and domain-general cognitive mechanisms (categorisation and memory).
Definition: There is a biologically constrained window (before puberty) during which language acquisition is most efficient.
Impact: If a child is not exposed to a language during this time, full native-like proficiency may be impossible, especially in syntax and phonology.
Evidence:
Case Studies: Genie, who was isolated until age 13, failed to acquire normal grammatical structures.
Late Sign Language Learners: Perform worse on syntactic tasks compared to those exposed from birth.
Neuroscientific Studies: Early language exposure leads to typical lateralisation and activation patterns in language-related brain areas; late learners often show more diffuse or right-hemisphere activation.
Cochlear Implant Studies: Children who receive implants earlier acquire language more effectively.
Conclusion: Brain plasticity for language is time-sensitive, and language input during the critical period is crucial for native-like mastery.
Complexity: More complex than L1 acquisition.
Evidence:
Earlier exposure may lead to more native-like proficiency, especially in pronunciation and grammar.
Johnson and Newport (1989) found a decline in grammatical performance among immigrants learning English after puberty.
Counter-Evidence: Birdsong and Molis (2001) found some adults achieved high levels of grammatical accuracy, suggesting compensation through motivation, input quality, and frequency of use.
Sensitive Period Model: While younger learners have advantages, successful L2 acquisition in adulthood is possible, particularly in immersive environments.
Bilingualism: Experience with multiple languages enhances cognitive flexibility and metalinguistic awareness.
Conclusion: Age interacts with social, psychological, and experiential variables in shaping L2 outcomes.
Mechanisms:
Lexical Route: Direct recognition of whole words from the mental lexicon; facilitates reading of irregular words (e.g., “choir,” “colonel”).
Non-Lexical Route: Grapheme-to-phoneme conversion; enables decoding of regular and novel words (e.g., “mint,” “flirp”).
Neuropsychological Evidence:
Surface Dyslexia: Damage to the lexical route results in difficulty reading irregular words, leading to regularisation errors (e.g., reading “pint” to rhyme with “mint”).
Phonological Dyslexia: Damage to the non-lexical route impairs ability to read non-words; familiar words remain accessible via the lexical route.
Functional Imaging Studies: Distinct neural pathways; temporal lobe regions support lexical access, and inferior parietal areas are involved in phonological conversion.
Location: Left fusiform gyrus.
Role: Facilitates rapid recognition of orthographic patterns.
Literate Individuals: Activates robustly during reading tasks, interfacing with phonological and semantic networks.
Illiterate Individuals: Shows reduced or absent activation during reading tasks.
Plasticity: Braille readers show VWFA responsiveness to tactile reading, indicating its tuning to symbolic input relevant to language.
Literacy Training: illiterate adults Leads to measurable increases in VWFA activity.
Conclusion: Reading recruits and repurposes existing brain regions through structured exposure and practice, highlighting that it is not an innate capacity.