A modified theory: early attenuation with later selection
Attention in Space
Evidence for early selective attenuation of information from visual cueing of spatial locations
Posner’s cueing paradigm
Fixate centrally
Present a central predictive cue
Involves voluntary, covert orienting
Valid Trials: Target appears at the cued location
Invalid Trials: Target appears opposite the cued location
Reaction Times (RTs) compared between Valid and invalid conditions
Attention in Space: Results
RTs were fastest to targets in the Valid condition
This suggested that stimuli presented at the valid target location were preferentially processed
Question: Was this selection at an early or late stage?
Attention in Space: ERP Evidence
ERP evidence for early selective attenuation (B&C p314)
Early visual evoked potentials, generated in extrastriate cortex, were of larger amplitude for targets at Valid locations than targets at Invalid locations
Preferential processing of attended input
See also B&C Fig 11.7 (Mangun & Hillyard, 1991)
Voluntary and Reflexive Orienting
Central, symbolic, cues evoke voluntary shifts of attention
This is slow
Validity effects show up with long Stimulus Onset Asynchronies (SOAs)
SOA is the delay between the presentation of the cue and the presentation of the target
Here it’s 800ms
Top-down control and Bottom-up processes
RT Table: Voluntary Orienting
The table shows reaction times (RTs) in milliseconds (ms) for valid and invalid conditions at short (100ms) and long (800ms) Stimulus Onset Asynchronies (SOAs).
A validity effect is observed only at the long SOA, demonstrating the slow nature of voluntary orienting.
Voluntary and Reflexive Orienting
Peripheral, non-symbolic, cues evoke reflexive shifts of attention
This is fast
Validity effects show up with short Stimulus Onset Asynchronies (SOAs)
SOA is the delay between the presentation of the cue and the presentation of the target
Top-down control and Bottom-up processes
RT Table: Reflexive Orienting
The table illustrates reaction times (RTs) in milliseconds (ms) for valid and invalid conditions at short (100ms) and long (800ms) Stimulus Onset Asynchronies (SOAs).
A validity effect is apparent usually only at short SOAs, reflecting the rapid nature of reflexive orienting.
Top-down vs. Bottom-up
Visual search - finding target features amongst distractors
Targets defined by single features can be identified preattentively (i.e. perceptually, without applying attention)
They ‘pop out’
Targets defined by feature conjunctions require serial attention (i.e. searching) to ‘bind’ the properties (features) of objects together
E.g. ‘redness’ and ‘O shape’
Visual Search Strategies
Strategies can increase the efficiency of serial visual search
Guided visual search results in a lesser impact of added distractors
Search is restricted to a subset of items, based on a single feature (target = red circle; exclude black items from serial search)
Top-down control and Bottom-up processes
Attention to Objects
Attention can also be ‘object based’ (B&C p311)
Objects can influence the orienting of attention in response to cues
RTs to Invalid-Same-Object trials faster than RTs to Invalid-Different-Object trials (Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(2), 161-177)
Attention to Objects
Subjects can selectively attend to stimulus class (e.g. face or place) and show correlated changes in inferotemporal regional brain processing
Wojciulik, Kanwisher, and Driver (1998). Journal of Neurophysiology, 79,1574-1578.
Attentional deficits can track objects
Disorders of Attention
Hemispatial neglect
Usually results from right parietal damage
Neglect of contralesional space (usually left)
Space can be ‘object defined’
Lesion overlay map
Disorders of Attention
Neglect can follow an object (object reference frame)
Deficit in attention to visual memories (not a memory deficit) (egocentric reference frame)
Imagine a piazza from either end
Brain Mechanisms of Attention
What is attention in the brain? (B&C p315-316)
Attention selects information for preferential processing in a variety of ways:
Spatial location - increased activation is observed in sensory areas that are organized with regard to space, such as early visual-processing areas, and regions that provide a spatial map of the world, such as parietal regions.
Item attributes - activation is increased in the brain region specialized for processing attended characteristic, such as motion-sensitive area MT.
Objects - increased activation is observed in areas that process objects, such as the ventral visual-processing stream.
Attention does not reside in one particular region of the brain but influences the processing of distinct brain modules, ramping up their activity if they are processing information that is attentionally relevant.
Attention is a modulatory process.
Brain Mechanisms of Attention
Attentional control and amplification of widespread processing
Multiple regions activated in fMRI studies of attentional orienting
Top-down modulation of early-processing areas via reciprocal connections through the thalamus (B&C p313 – 315)
Synchronisation of neural firing prior to stimulus input? (suggested by regional fMRI activation)
Neuron’s preferred stimuli may become more specific with increased attention (i.e. the ‘tuning curve’ narrows – suggested by recordings in monkeys)
Brain Mechanisms: Attentional Networks
Three main attentional networks in the brain.
Alerting - maintaining a high state of sensitivity to incoming stimuli.
Orienting - attending to the source of a sensory signal.
Executive - directs attention according to an individual’s goals
B&C Fig. 11.1 (Posner & Rothbart, 2007)
Conclusions
Selection happens at both early and late stages
Attention is modulated by both top-down and bottom-up processes
Attention can be paid to locations, features or to objects
Multiple cortical and subcortical brain areas are involved