A

Theories of Positive Reinforcement and Neuroscience

Theories of Positive Reinforcement

Introduction to Positive Reinforcement

  • Positive reinforcement is a fundamental concept in behavioral psychology, exploring why certain actions are reinforced through rewards.

Prominent Theories of Positive Reinforcement

Hull's Drive-Reduction Theory

  • This theory attributes the effectiveness of reinforcers to the reduction of a drive or motivational state (e.g., hunger) stemming from deprivation.

  • Key Points:

    • An event provides reinforcement if it reduces one or more drives, aligning behavior with satisfying physiological needs.

    • Critics argue that many reinforcers (e.g., praise, bonuses) do not reduce physiological needs, and examples exist where reinforcement occurs without direct drive reduction, such as copulation without ejaculation in male rats.

Relative Value Theory and the Premack Principle

Relative Value Theory
  • The theory posits that reinforcers are essentially behaviors rather than mere stimuli.

  • It determines a reinforcer's effectiveness based on its probability relative to other behaviors.

  • Illustration:

    • Engaging in study methods that lead to good exam grades reinforces those study behaviors, as they hold a relative value.

  • This theory claims that behaviors have varying probabilities of occurrence, resulting in differential values for reinforcing behaviors.

The Premack Principle
  • This principle outlines that the opportunity to engage in a high-probability behavior after a low-probability behavior acts as a reinforcer.

  • Expression of the principle:

    • For instance, if a rat is deprived of water, drinking water reinforces the behavior of running, while in exercise-deprived conditions, running may reinforce drinking.

    • Everyday examples:

    • First graders cleaning their workspace to gain extra recess time.

    • College students completing quizzes to watch Netflix.

  • Important Note:

    • The Premack Principle does not invoke hypothetical concepts like drives, but it struggles to explain secondary reinforcers (e.g., money is not a behavior).

Response-Deprivation Theory

  • This theory posits that a behavior is reinforcing in direct correlation to how much an organism has been deprived of performing that behavior compared to its baseline frequency.

  • It builds upon Premack’s work but emphasizes behavior occurrence below its preferred rate rather than the relative value among different behaviors.

  • Example:

    • A student who hasn't had time to clean their room while studying may find cleaning reinforcing after exams when they have the chance again.

  • Critique:

    • Response-deprivation theory also faces challenges in explaining secondary reinforcers.

Theories of Avoidance: Negative Reinforcement

Two-Process Theory

  • Suggests that both Pavlovian (classical conditioning) and Operant processes are involved in avoidance learning.

One-Process Theory

  • States that only operant processes are at work in avoidance learning.

  • These concepts will be revisited in future discussions throughout the semester.

Neuromechanics of Operant Learning

Skinner's Radical Behaviorism

  • Skinner's perspective maintains a clear distinction between behaviorism and physiological processes, focusing exclusively on the environment and observable behaviors.

  • Key Assertions:

    • Experiential factors are vital in shaping behavior, thus facilitating a natural science study of these influences.

    • It does not imply that organisms are blank slates unaffected by biological or genetic predispositions.

Donald Olding (D.O.) Hebb

  • A contemporary Canadian psychologist from the 1940s, recognized for his research into neuronal functions during learning.

  • Notable Contributions:

    • Developed the theory of Hebbian Learning, emphasizing how neuronal connections strengthen through repeated stimulation.

    • Acknowledged as a pioneer in neuropsychology and neural networks, distinguishing learning's neural mechanisms.

James Olds and Peter Milner (1954)

  • Experiments led by two students of Hebb discovered electrical stimulation in the basal forebrain of rats acted as a reinforcer.

  • Methodology:

    • Stimulation was provided whenever a rat reached a specific location in a maze, reinforcing the behavior and demonstrating learned behavior through altered location in the maze.

    • Notably, the rat repeatedly returned to the stimulation site once it was altered.

    • Led to the ability to train a rat to press a lever for stimulation within hours, establishing foundational methodologies in reward learning.

Electrical Brain Stimulation

  • Olds and Milner conducted extensive research across various brain regions to identify locations yielding similar effects.

  • Discovery:

    • Specific brain areas elicited strong self-stimulation in rats.

    • Example: A rat pressed a lever 7,500 times in 12 hours for stimulation in the septal area, beneath the corpus callosum.

  • Published findings highlighted the potential for physiological reward mechanisms research, prompting Skinner's clarification on Reinforcement versus Reward Learning.

Intracranial Self-Stimulation (ICSS)

  • ICSS offers distinct advantages in laboratory settings:

    • It directly activates brain reward systems, circumventing much of the stimulus input needed in natural contexts.

    • No prerequisite for establishing operations, such as correcting motivational deficits from food or water deprivation.

    • Allows complete control over reward value.

  • This work laid crucial groundwork for the understanding of reinforcement learning in the brain, with additional discussion on controversial applications of these findings.

Dopamine and Reward

Mapping Reward Areas in the Brain

  • Follow-up studies discovered that sensitive areas related to reward were located in the Medial Forebrain Bundle (MFB), integral for transmitting signals between the Substantia Nigra/Ventral Tegmental Area (SN/VTA) and the lateral hypothalamus.

  • Findings:

    • Some MFB areas were so sensitive that rats preferred stimulation over basic needs like food or sex.

    • Stimulation activated dopamine neurons; blockers of dopamine hindered learning and could negate interest in previously addictive stimuli, providing insight into addiction mechanisms.

Dopamine Pathways in Reward Learning

  • Dopamine plays numerous roles in the brain, with four key pathways primarily involved in reward learning:

    • Mesocortical Pathway: Predominantly a reward pathway.

    • Mesolimbic Pathway: Vital for emotional experiences tied to reward.

  • Disruption of the VTA to NAcc pathway adversely affects learning mechanisms, illustrated by a cessation in lever pressing associated with rewards following disruption in addicted rats.

Skinner's Behavioral Atoms

Evolution of Operant Learning Concept

  • Towards the later stages of Skinner's career, he proposed a refined view of operant learning, identifying it as a sequence of smaller behavioral units termed "Response Elements" or "Atoms of Behavior."

  • Significance:

    • These small units, rather than entire behavioral responses, constitute the functional units of reinforcement.

Neuroscientific Investigation of Behavioral Atoms

Experimentation by Stein et al. (1989;1994)
  • Stein interpreted Skinner’s idea, theorizing a biological basis for atoms of behavior through dopaminergic neuron activity.

  • Utilizing an "in vitro reinforcement" paradigm, he evaluated electrical activity patterns in hippocampal slices, reinforcing bursts with dopamine.

  • Results:

    • Reinforcement altered electrical burst patterns compared to non-contingent dopamine delivery at the same dosage.

    • Similar results were evidenced in clusters of individual neurons, indicating dopamine receptors function as the atoms of behavior that drive learning dynamics.

Behavioral Neuroscience

  • The field encompasses the biological roots of behavior in both human and animal subjects.

  • Definition:

    • More formally, it applies biological principles to explore physiological, genetic, and developmental mechanisms behind behavioral patterns (also recognized as biological psychology, biopsychology, or psychobiology).

  • Scope:

    • It's an expansive research area informing numerous issues regarding both typical and atypical human behaviors.