Untitled Flashcards Set
Psychophysical property | Property that depends on our psychological interpretation of it |
Spectrum of light wavelengths we can see | 400-700 |
3 steps to color perception | Detection: our S, M, and L cones seeing color Discrimination: tell the difference between one wavelength and another
Appearance: assign perceived colors to lights and surfaces that are stable in different lighting conditions |
3 types of cone photoreceptors | S blue, M green, and L red |
L cone peak sensitivity | 565 nm - corresponds to yellow |
Why do we call cones S, M, L and not B, G, R | Because there is a range of wavelengths that are B, G and R |
Photopic | High light, bright enough to saturate rods and stimulate cones |
Scotopic | Low light, bright enough to stimulate rods but not enough for cones |
Rhodopsin | Rods, 100x more sensitive to light than cones All rods have same sensitivity to different wavelengths of light |
Principle of univariance | Multiple wavelength and intensity combinations can produce the same color |
How to solve univariance | Have 3 cones |
Trichromatic theory of color vision | Color is defined with three numbers |
Metamers | Two different wavelength combinations that result in the same color |
Why is it called Young-Helmholtz theory? | They both discovered it independently |
James Maxwell color matching technique | Still used today |
Additive color mixing | When colors are put together and their effects enhance each other. ex: how we see white |
Subtractive color mixing | When colors are put together and their effects subtract from each other. ex: combine blue and yellow to make green |
LGN | Has cells stimulated by spots of light Center-surround organization |
Cone-opponent cell | Neuron whose output is based on a difference between sets of cones |
Color space | 3D space that describes all colors |
RGB color space | Amount of R, G, B |
HSB color space | Hue: color Saturation: how intense color is Brightness: how much light |
Nonspectral colors | Colors that we can only see as a combination of multiple wavelengths |
How do we perceive purple? | Activation of S and L cones |
Opponent color theory | Perception of color depends on the output of three mechanisms, each based on opponency between two colors |
LGN color activation | Some LGN cells activated by L in center, inhibited by M in surround (red vs. green) Some LGN cells activated by S in center, inhibited by (L+M) in surround (blue vs. yellow) …& vice versa |
Hering "illegal" color combinations | Red and green, blue and yellow, black and white Opposite colors on the color wheel |
Hue cancellation experiments | Start with a color, the goal is to end with a pure version of that color by shining different lights |
Unique hues | Red, blue, green, yellow. Colors that can be described with just one term |
Achromatopsia | Fully color blind from brain damage. Can find boundaries between color regions |
Cultural relativism | Different cultures describe colors differently, perception determined by cultural environment |
Basic color terms | The common color terms that people use |
Color category boundaries | if something crosses the color category boundary, it’s easy for us to determine which color is different from others |
Why is color blindness more common in men? | M and L cones on X chromosome |
Color anomalous | Color blindness. Can still make discriminations from wavelength |
Deuteranope | Missing M, yellow-green-red messed up |
Protanope | Missing L, yellow-green-red messed up |
Tritanope | Missing S |
Cone monochromat | Having one cone type |
Rod monochromat | Having no cone types |
Anomia | Inability to name objects or colors even though you can recognize them. Due to brain damage |
Synesthesia | One stimulus automatically activates experience of another stimulus that isn’t present |
Color contrast | Two colors next to each other enhance one another, color opponency |
Color assimilation | Two colors next to each other dullen one another |
Unrelated color | Color that can be experience in isolation |
Related color | Color that is only seen in relation to other colors (ex: brown or grey) |
Negative afterimages | When you stare at an image and look away, you will see an image with the opposite colors |
Illuminant - broadband | Contains many different wavelengths |
Surface - broadband | Reflects many different wavelengths |
Why is color good for survival? | Determining if food is ripe and finding mates |
How does flavor of food get affected by color? | Ex: white wine dyed pink tastes more like rose than undyed wine |
Dogs color vision | Dichromatic |
Chickens color vision | Tetrachromatic |
Mantis shrimp color vision | 12 cones |
Silver spiny fin fish color vision | 38 rods |
Bird/reptile color vision | Colored oil on photoreceptors tunes them to different wavelengths |
Dots vary in hue | Can distinguish |
Dots vary in saturation | Can’t distinguish |
How does attention select a group of items, color-wise? | Based on hue |
Realism | The world exists |
Positivist | The world depends on evidence of senses |
Euclidean Geometry | Parallel lines that extend in space don’t cross, all angles of a triangle add to 180 |
Are images presented onto the retina euclidean? | No |
Probability summation | Increased probability of seeing stimulus when you have two or more samples |
Binocular summation | Combination of information from both eyes that makes you better at something that if you had only one eye |
Binocular disparity | The two eyes see slightly different things |
Stereopsis | Perception of depth by using both eyes |
Monocular depth cue | A depth cue you can see with one eye |
Binocular depth cue | A depth cue you can see with two eyes |
Occlusion | Something in front of something else |
Metrical depth cue | A cue where you know exact sizes Cells code precise distance of feature from the plane of fixation |
Nonmetrical depth cue | A cue that gives you information about relative depth (ex: occlusion) Cells code whether feature is in front of or behind plane of fixation |
Relative size | Knowing the size of something compared to something else without knowing their actual sizes |
Relative height | When objects touch the ground, those higher in the visual field appear to be farther away |
Texture gradient | When things are bigger and get smaller in a photo, we assume the bigger things are closer to us |
Familiar size | Knowing approximate size of something familiar |
Relative metrical cue | A cue that can specify that object A is 2x further away than B without knowing absolute distance to A or B |
Absolute metrical depth cue | Provides quantifiable information about distance in the third dimension |
Aerial perspective | Depth cue based on implicit understanding that light is scattered by atmosphere |
Linear perspective | Lines in 3d appear to converge into 2d as they extend in the distance |
Vanishing point | The point where parallel lines converge |
Pictorial depth cue | Depth cues artists use to make photos look 3D |
Anamorphosis | Create a 2D image so distorted it only looks correct from a special angle |
Motion parallax | When something is closer to us we see it as moving faster |
Accommodation | Lens changing shape to let more or less light in |
Convergence | Eyes going inward, ex: crossing |
Divergence | Eyes going outward |
Corresponding retinal points | Points on the two different retinas that allow someone to see one thing |
Horopter | Location of objects whose images lie on corresponding points |
Vieth-Muller circle | Location of objects whose images lie on corresponding points |
We can consider horopters and Vieth-Mullers to be... | The same |
Panum's fusional area | Region of space in front of and behind horopter where binocular single vision is possible |
Diplopia | Seeing two things because they’re not on corresponding points |
Crossed disparity | Objects in front of plane of horopter. Image is to the left in the right eye and to the right in the left eye |
Uncrossed disparity | Objects behind plane of horopter. Image is to the left in the left eye and to the right in the right eye |
Stereoscope | Device that shows a different photo to each eye |
Free fusion | Converging or diverging eyes to view a stereogram without a stereoscope |
Stereoblindness | Unable to use binocular disparity as a depth cue |
Does binocular vision develop during critical period? | No |
Brock string | Hold one end of a string to a wall and the other to someone’s nose. It has beads on it and they have to focus on the beads to practice their stereoacuity |
Random dot stereogram | Stereogram made of random dots, has no depth cues |
Cyclopean | Stimuli defined by binocular disparity alone |
How do 3D movies work? | Have two slightly different images in each eye |
Correspondence problem | Figuring out which bit in image in left eye should be matched with which bit in image in right eye |
Correspondence problem solution | Blurring the image, uniqueness constraint, continuity constraint |
Uniqueness constraint | Observation that feature in the world is represented once in each retinal image |
Continuity constraint | neighboring points in the world lie at similar distances from viewer |
Are binocular neurons tuned to binocular disparity? | Yes |
Binocular rivalry and when do we get it? | Competition between two eyes for control of visual perception, get it when two different stimuli are presented to the two eyes |
Stereoacuity | Smallest binocular disparity that can generate a sensation of depth |
Dichoptic | Presentation of two stimuli, one to each eye |
Onset of stereopsis | 3-5 months |
Development of stereoacuity | 6-7 months |
Strabismus | Misalignment of eyes |
Esotropia | One eye turns inward |
Exotropia | One eye turns outward |
Esotropia during critical period | If it’s during the critical period, it leads to stereoblindness |
Selective attention | Attention involved when processing is restricted to a subset of the possible stimuli |
Internal attention | Paying attention to something internally. Ex: repeating a sentence in your head |
Overt attention | Turning an organ to something Ex: moving head to look at something |
Covert attention | Paying attention to something without an outward sign that you’re doing so |
Invalid vs. valid cues | Invalid: doesn’t cue to right direction Valid: does cue to right direction |
Posner cueing paradigm | Showed symbolic and peripheral cues |
Peripheral vs. symbolic cue | Peripheral: cue directly shows where something will be. Symbolic: cue indirectly shows where something will be. Takes longer. Ex: if this dot turns green, the image is on the left. |
Inhibition of return | Once we pay attention to something, our brain prevents us from going back to the area immediately after, good for visual searches |
Spotlight model | We have a certain point of fixation that moves as we move around |
Zoom lens model | Our point of fixation gets bigger or smaller depending on the size of the area we’re scanning |
Visual search target, distractor, set size | Target: what we’re looking for Distractor: everything in the set that isn’t the target Set size: the amount of things in the set |
Search slope | Time/object; ms/object |
Large search slope implications | Inefficient search |
Feature search | Looking for something that is defined by one feature |
Salience | Vividness of stimulus relative to its neighbors |
Parallel | Processing multiple things at once |
Serial self-terminating search | Looking from item to item and when we find it, we stop looking |
Archerfish study | Archerfish spray their prey. They were trained to spray a screen if they thought a fish was on the screen, and their searching skills are similar to humans. |
Guided search | Restricted to a subset of possible items based on information about its basic features |
Conjunction search | Looking for something that differs based on multiple features. |
Scene-based guidance | When we have previous knowledge of something that helps us search |
Binding problem | Issue with tying different attributes of visual stimuli to appropriate object until we see a unified object Different neurons perceive different things |
Feature integration theory | Limited set of basic features can be processed parallely preattentively |
Preattentive stage | Processing of a stimulus before selective attention is deployed to stimulus |
Illusory conjunction | Erroneously thinking two features are attributed to the same object |
Rapid serial visual presentation | Showing a stream of images at once. 8/second Used to study temporal dynamics of visual attention |
Attentional blink | If we see one target and another right after, we will not process the second one |
Second target is missed if its within __ of first target | 200-500 ms |
People who have reduced attentional blink | First shooter video games |
Marvin Chun's fishing metaphor | Fishing, as you pick up a fish you are unable to pick up other fish for a few seconds and may miss some |
Attention neural activity | When you pay attention to different things, different parts of the brain work |
Attention used in fMRI | Attentional neural activity can be used in fMRI |
Fusiform face area | In fusiform gyrus that specializes in perceiving faces |
Parahippocampal place area | In temporal lobe that specializes in perceiving places |
Three ways cell response can be changed by attention | Response enhancement, sharper tuning, altered tuning |
Visual-field defect | Portion of visual field with no vision or abnormal vision Damage to visual nervous system Damage to parietal lobe: visual field defect where one side of world is ignored |
Neglect | When you ignore one side of something |
Cotralesional vs. ipsilesional | Visual field on opposite side of brain lesion vs on same side of brain lesion |
Neglect task example | Ask someone to draw a house, they will only draw one side |
Is neglect on one side of object or visual field? | Can be either |
Extinction | Can’t perceive a stimulus to one side of the point of fixation in the presence of another stimulus |
Selective attention and ADHD | Attention deficit disorders that don’t affect visual attention |
Selective pathway | Analyzes specific objects, small range Goes through bottleneck |
Nonselective pathway | Analyzes scenes, gists Doesn’t go through bottleneck |
Ensemble statistics | Average and distribution of properties over a set of objects or a region in a scene, nonselective |
Spatial layout | Layout of something |
What pathway analyzes spatial layout? | Nonselective pathway |
Brandy memory task | People looked at 2500 objects, then had to choose which objects they had previously seen. Different types > same category > different states |
Change blindness | Not noticing a change in a scene |
Inattentional blindness | Not noticing the addition of a new stimulus in a scene because you’re focusing on something else |
Motion aftereffects | When you look at something in motion for a while and then look at something stationary, it will look like that thing is moving |
Interocular transfer | Transfer from one eye to another |
MAE must be in neurons that... | Respond to both eyes |
Which brain region is MAE in/after? | V1 |
Describe motion detector neurons | Neuron → D cells (detection) → X cells (multiplication) → M cells (ensure both neurons are responding) |
Apparent motion | If photos are flashed rapidly of someone in different positions, it will look like they are moving |
What does apparent motion tell us about the motion detector circuit? | We don’t need actual motion for the motion detector circuit to be activated |
Correspondence problem | Not being able to tell which movement corresponds to what frame |
Aperture problem | If we see something moving through an aperture, we can’t actually tell which direction it’s moving |
Global motion of the object | Allows us to tell which direction something is moving. Global motion is the direction the every aperture is moving in |
Lesion in magnocellular layers | Problems processing large, quick moving objects |
MT role in motion perception | Processing motion |
Akinetopsia | Inability to process motion, seeing things in stop-motion |
What causes akinetopsia? | Damage to the MT |
Newsome and Pare study | Taught monkeys to respond to moving dots. When their MT was lesioned, they needed 10x more dots to respond. However, since lesioning is invasive it’s better to electrically stimulate portions of their brain, which is also better because you can bias it towards brain regions that respond to certain directions. |
First-order motion | Motion defined by changes in luminance |
Second-order motion | Motion defined by changes in texture. Usually only seen in labs |
Optic array | Lights in the world around us |
Optic flow | Points in our vision moving as we move |
Optic flow field | Flow pattern that happens when we are moving forward rapidly in space |
Focus of expansion | If we go straight into the horizon, things on the sides of us will look like they’re expanding |
Time to collision | We can tell time to collision using tau |
Tau | Ratio of size of object on retina to rate that object is expanding |
Troxler fading | When we stare at something stationary, stationary things in our periphery will disappear |
Motion induced blindness | When we stare at something in motion, stationary things in our periphery will disappear |
Smooth pursuit | When we look at something in motion and follow it with our eyes, our eyes move smoothly |
Saccade | When we move our eyes to another point of fixation it is very jerky |
Eye muscles | 6 muscles, in pairs |
Superior colliculus | Part of the midbrain that processes and controls movements |
Saccadic suppression | This reduces the sensitivity that happens when we have saccades |
Efference copy | When there is movement, the motion system sends a copy to other cortexes |
Comparator | Comparator receives efference copy and motor system copy and compare them. if they’re the same, movement is of eyes. If they’re different, movement is physical. |
Dynamic remapping | When our mind predicts a saccade, the neurons start remapping towards that area so we start processing that location before our eyes move to it |
When do people get reflexive eye movements | Infant at birth |
When do people get adult sensitivity to motion | 3-4 years |
When do people get sensitivity to motion defined form and biological motion | 4+ years |
Psychophysical property | Property that depends on our psychological interpretation of it |
Spectrum of light wavelengths we can see | 400-700 |
3 steps to color perception | Detection: our S, M, and L cones seeing color Discrimination: tell the difference between one wavelength and another
Appearance: assign perceived colors to lights and surfaces that are stable in different lighting conditions |
3 types of cone photoreceptors | S blue, M green, and L red |
L cone peak sensitivity | 565 nm - corresponds to yellow |
Why do we call cones S, M, L and not B, G, R | Because there is a range of wavelengths that are B, G and R |
Photopic | High light, bright enough to saturate rods and stimulate cones |
Scotopic | Low light, bright enough to stimulate rods but not enough for cones |
Rhodopsin | Rods, 100x more sensitive to light than cones All rods have same sensitivity to different wavelengths of light |
Principle of univariance | Multiple wavelength and intensity combinations can produce the same color |
How to solve univariance | Have 3 cones |
Trichromatic theory of color vision | Color is defined with three numbers |
Metamers | Two different wavelength combinations that result in the same color |
Why is it called Young-Helmholtz theory? | They both discovered it independently |
James Maxwell color matching technique | Still used today |
Additive color mixing | When colors are put together and their effects enhance each other. ex: how we see white |
Subtractive color mixing | When colors are put together and their effects subtract from each other. ex: combine blue and yellow to make green |
LGN | Has cells stimulated by spots of light Center-surround organization |
Cone-opponent cell | Neuron whose output is based on a difference between sets of cones |
Color space | 3D space that describes all colors |
RGB color space | Amount of R, G, B |
HSB color space | Hue: color Saturation: how intense color is Brightness: how much light |
Nonspectral colors | Colors that we can only see as a combination of multiple wavelengths |
How do we perceive purple? | Activation of S and L cones |
Opponent color theory | Perception of color depends on the output of three mechanisms, each based on opponency between two colors |
LGN color activation | Some LGN cells activated by L in center, inhibited by M in surround (red vs. green) Some LGN cells activated by S in center, inhibited by (L+M) in surround (blue vs. yellow) …& vice versa |
Hering "illegal" color combinations | Red and green, blue and yellow, black and white Opposite colors on the color wheel |
Hue cancellation experiments | Start with a color, the goal is to end with a pure version of that color by shining different lights |
Unique hues | Red, blue, green, yellow. Colors that can be described with just one term |
Achromatopsia | Fully color blind from brain damage. Can find boundaries between color regions |
Cultural relativism | Different cultures describe colors differently, perception determined by cultural environment |
Basic color terms | The common color terms that people use |
Color category boundaries | if something crosses the color category boundary, it’s easy for us to determine which color is different from others |
Why is color blindness more common in men? | M and L cones on X chromosome |
Color anomalous | Color blindness. Can still make discriminations from wavelength |
Deuteranope | Missing M, yellow-green-red messed up |
Protanope | Missing L, yellow-green-red messed up |
Tritanope | Missing S |
Cone monochromat | Having one cone type |
Rod monochromat | Having no cone types |
Anomia | Inability to name objects or colors even though you can recognize them. Due to brain damage |
Synesthesia | One stimulus automatically activates experience of another stimulus that isn’t present |
Color contrast | Two colors next to each other enhance one another, color opponency |
Color assimilation | Two colors next to each other dullen one another |
Unrelated color | Color that can be experience in isolation |
Related color | Color that is only seen in relation to other colors (ex: brown or grey) |
Negative afterimages | When you stare at an image and look away, you will see an image with the opposite colors |
Illuminant - broadband | Contains many different wavelengths |
Surface - broadband | Reflects many different wavelengths |
Why is color good for survival? | Determining if food is ripe and finding mates |
How does flavor of food get affected by color? | Ex: white wine dyed pink tastes more like rose than undyed wine |
Dogs color vision | Dichromatic |
Chickens color vision | Tetrachromatic |
Mantis shrimp color vision | 12 cones |
Silver spiny fin fish color vision | 38 rods |
Bird/reptile color vision | Colored oil on photoreceptors tunes them to different wavelengths |
Dots vary in hue | Can distinguish |
Dots vary in saturation | Can’t distinguish |
How does attention select a group of items, color-wise? | Based on hue |
Realism | The world exists |
Positivist | The world depends on evidence of senses |
Euclidean Geometry | Parallel lines that extend in space don’t cross, all angles of a triangle add to 180 |
Are images presented onto the retina euclidean? | No |
Probability summation | Increased probability of seeing stimulus when you have two or more samples |
Binocular summation | Combination of information from both eyes that makes you better at something that if you had only one eye |
Binocular disparity | The two eyes see slightly different things |
Stereopsis | Perception of depth by using both eyes |
Monocular depth cue | A depth cue you can see with one eye |
Binocular depth cue | A depth cue you can see with two eyes |
Occlusion | Something in front of something else |
Metrical depth cue | A cue where you know exact sizes Cells code precise distance of feature from the plane of fixation |
Nonmetrical depth cue | A cue that gives you information about relative depth (ex: occlusion) Cells code whether feature is in front of or behind plane of fixation |
Relative size | Knowing the size of something compared to something else without knowing their actual sizes |
Relative height | When objects touch the ground, those higher in the visual field appear to be farther away |
Texture gradient | When things are bigger and get smaller in a photo, we assume the bigger things are closer to us |
Familiar size | Knowing approximate size of something familiar |
Relative metrical cue | A cue that can specify that object A is 2x further away than B without knowing absolute distance to A or B |
Absolute metrical depth cue | Provides quantifiable information about distance in the third dimension |
Aerial perspective | Depth cue based on implicit understanding that light is scattered by atmosphere |
Linear perspective | Lines in 3d appear to converge into 2d as they extend in the distance |
Vanishing point | The point where parallel lines converge |
Pictorial depth cue | Depth cues artists use to make photos look 3D |
Anamorphosis | Create a 2D image so distorted it only looks correct from a special angle |
Motion parallax | When something is closer to us we see it as moving faster |
Accommodation | Lens changing shape to let more or less light in |
Convergence | Eyes going inward, ex: crossing |
Divergence | Eyes going outward |
Corresponding retinal points | Points on the two different retinas that allow someone to see one thing |
Horopter | Location of objects whose images lie on corresponding points |
Vieth-Muller circle | Location of objects whose images lie on corresponding points |
We can consider horopters and Vieth-Mullers to be... | The same |
Panum's fusional area | Region of space in front of and behind horopter where binocular single vision is possible |
Diplopia | Seeing two things because they’re not on corresponding points |
Crossed disparity | Objects in front of plane of horopter. Image is to the left in the right eye and to the right in the left eye |
Uncrossed disparity | Objects behind plane of horopter. Image is to the left in the left eye and to the right in the right eye |
Stereoscope | Device that shows a different photo to each eye |
Free fusion | Converging or diverging eyes to view a stereogram without a stereoscope |
Stereoblindness | Unable to use binocular disparity as a depth cue |
Does binocular vision develop during critical period? | No |
Brock string | Hold one end of a string to a wall and the other to someone’s nose. It has beads on it and they have to focus on the beads to practice their stereoacuity |
Random dot stereogram | Stereogram made of random dots, has no depth cues |
Cyclopean | Stimuli defined by binocular disparity alone |
How do 3D movies work? | Have two slightly different images in each eye |
Correspondence problem | Figuring out which bit in image in left eye should be matched with which bit in image in right eye |
Correspondence problem solution | Blurring the image, uniqueness constraint, continuity constraint |
Uniqueness constraint | Observation that feature in the world is represented once in each retinal image |
Continuity constraint | neighboring points in the world lie at similar distances from viewer |
Are binocular neurons tuned to binocular disparity? | Yes |
Binocular rivalry and when do we get it? | Competition between two eyes for control of visual perception, get it when two different stimuli are presented to the two eyes |
Stereoacuity | Smallest binocular disparity that can generate a sensation of depth |
Dichoptic | Presentation of two stimuli, one to each eye |
Onset of stereopsis | 3-5 months |
Development of stereoacuity | 6-7 months |
Strabismus | Misalignment of eyes |
Esotropia | One eye turns inward |
Exotropia | One eye turns outward |
Esotropia during critical period | If it’s during the critical period, it leads to stereoblindness |
Selective attention | Attention involved when processing is restricted to a subset of the possible stimuli |
Internal attention | Paying attention to something internally. Ex: repeating a sentence in your head |
Overt attention | Turning an organ to something Ex: moving head to look at something |
Covert attention | Paying attention to something without an outward sign that you’re doing so |
Invalid vs. valid cues | Invalid: doesn’t cue to right direction Valid: does cue to right direction |
Posner cueing paradigm | Showed symbolic and peripheral cues |
Peripheral vs. symbolic cue | Peripheral: cue directly shows where something will be. Symbolic: cue indirectly shows where something will be. Takes longer. Ex: if this dot turns green, the image is on the left. |
Inhibition of return | Once we pay attention to something, our brain prevents us from going back to the area immediately after, good for visual searches |
Spotlight model | We have a certain point of fixation that moves as we move around |
Zoom lens model | Our point of fixation gets bigger or smaller depending on the size of the area we’re scanning |
Visual search target, distractor, set size | Target: what we’re looking for Distractor: everything in the set that isn’t the target Set size: the amount of things in the set |
Search slope | Time/object; ms/object |
Large search slope implications | Inefficient search |
Feature search | Looking for something that is defined by one feature |
Salience | Vividness of stimulus relative to its neighbors |
Parallel | Processing multiple things at once |
Serial self-terminating search | Looking from item to item and when we find it, we stop looking |
Archerfish study | Archerfish spray their prey. They were trained to spray a screen if they thought a fish was on the screen, and their searching skills are similar to humans. |
Guided search | Restricted to a subset of possible items based on information about its basic features |
Conjunction search | Looking for something that differs based on multiple features. |
Scene-based guidance | When we have previous knowledge of something that helps us search |
Binding problem | Issue with tying different attributes of visual stimuli to appropriate object until we see a unified object Different neurons perceive different things |
Feature integration theory | Limited set of basic features can be processed parallely preattentively |
Preattentive stage | Processing of a stimulus before selective attention is deployed to stimulus |
Illusory conjunction | Erroneously thinking two features are attributed to the same object |
Rapid serial visual presentation | Showing a stream of images at once. 8/second Used to study temporal dynamics of visual attention |
Attentional blink | If we see one target and another right after, we will not process the second one |
Second target is missed if its within __ of first target | 200-500 ms |
People who have reduced attentional blink | First shooter video games |
Marvin Chun's fishing metaphor | Fishing, as you pick up a fish you are unable to pick up other fish for a few seconds and may miss some |
Attention neural activity | When you pay attention to different things, different parts of the brain work |
Attention used in fMRI | Attentional neural activity can be used in fMRI |
Fusiform face area | In fusiform gyrus that specializes in perceiving faces |
Parahippocampal place area | In temporal lobe that specializes in perceiving places |
Three ways cell response can be changed by attention | Response enhancement, sharper tuning, altered tuning |
Visual-field defect | Portion of visual field with no vision or abnormal vision Damage to visual nervous system Damage to parietal lobe: visual field defect where one side of world is ignored |
Neglect | When you ignore one side of something |
Cotralesional vs. ipsilesional | Visual field on opposite side of brain lesion vs on same side of brain lesion |
Neglect task example | Ask someone to draw a house, they will only draw one side |
Is neglect on one side of object or visual field? | Can be either |
Extinction | Can’t perceive a stimulus to one side of the point of fixation in the presence of another stimulus |
Selective attention and ADHD | Attention deficit disorders that don’t affect visual attention |
Selective pathway | Analyzes specific objects, small range Goes through bottleneck |
Nonselective pathway | Analyzes scenes, gists Doesn’t go through bottleneck |
Ensemble statistics | Average and distribution of properties over a set of objects or a region in a scene, nonselective |
Spatial layout | Layout of something |
What pathway analyzes spatial layout? | Nonselective pathway |
Brandy memory task | People looked at 2500 objects, then had to choose which objects they had previously seen. Different types > same category > different states |
Change blindness | Not noticing a change in a scene |
Inattentional blindness | Not noticing the addition of a new stimulus in a scene because you’re focusing on something else |
Motion aftereffects | When you look at something in motion for a while and then look at something stationary, it will look like that thing is moving |
Interocular transfer | Transfer from one eye to another |
MAE must be in neurons that... | Respond to both eyes |
Which brain region is MAE in/after? | V1 |
Describe motion detector neurons | Neuron → D cells (detection) → X cells (multiplication) → M cells (ensure both neurons are responding) |
Apparent motion | If photos are flashed rapidly of someone in different positions, it will look like they are moving |
What does apparent motion tell us about the motion detector circuit? | We don’t need actual motion for the motion detector circuit to be activated |
Correspondence problem | Not being able to tell which movement corresponds to what frame |
Aperture problem | If we see something moving through an aperture, we can’t actually tell which direction it’s moving |
Global motion of the object | Allows us to tell which direction something is moving. Global motion is the direction the every aperture is moving in |
Lesion in magnocellular layers | Problems processing large, quick moving objects |
MT role in motion perception | Processing motion |
Akinetopsia | Inability to process motion, seeing things in stop-motion |
What causes akinetopsia? | Damage to the MT |
Newsome and Pare study | Taught monkeys to respond to moving dots. When their MT was lesioned, they needed 10x more dots to respond. However, since lesioning is invasive it’s better to electrically stimulate portions of their brain, which is also better because you can bias it towards brain regions that respond to certain directions. |
First-order motion | Motion defined by changes in luminance |
Second-order motion | Motion defined by changes in texture. Usually only seen in labs |
Optic array | Lights in the world around us |
Optic flow | Points in our vision moving as we move |
Optic flow field | Flow pattern that happens when we are moving forward rapidly in space |
Focus of expansion | If we go straight into the horizon, things on the sides of us will look like they’re expanding |
Time to collision | We can tell time to collision using tau |
Tau | Ratio of size of object on retina to rate that object is expanding |
Troxler fading | When we stare at something stationary, stationary things in our periphery will disappear |
Motion induced blindness | When we stare at something in motion, stationary things in our periphery will disappear |
Smooth pursuit | When we look at something in motion and follow it with our eyes, our eyes move smoothly |
Saccade | When we move our eyes to another point of fixation it is very jerky |
Eye muscles | 6 muscles, in pairs |
Superior colliculus | Part of the midbrain that processes and controls movements |
Saccadic suppression | This reduces the sensitivity that happens when we have saccades |
Efference copy | When there is movement, the motion system sends a copy to other cortexes |
Comparator | Comparator receives efference copy and motor system copy and compare them. if they’re the same, movement is of eyes. If they’re different, movement is physical. |
Dynamic remapping | When our mind predicts a saccade, the neurons start remapping towards that area so we start processing that location before our eyes move to it |
When do people get reflexive eye movements | Infant at birth |
When do people get adult sensitivity to motion | 3-4 years |
When do people get sensitivity to motion defined form and biological motion | 4+ years |