Notes on Why Some Terrorist Attacks Receive More Media Attention Than Others
Introduction
President Trump stated that media neglect to report some terrorist attacks.
His administration released a list of purportedly underreported attacks with predominantly Muslim perpetrators.
Reporters and academics dismissed Trump’s claim, demonstrating that these attacks were often covered extensively.
The study argues that media do not cover some terrorist attacks at all, while others receive disproportionate coverage.
Research Question
Why do some terrorist attacks receive more media coverage than others?
Media are naturally drawn to covering conflicts, especially shocking or sensational ones (Tuman, 2010).
Terrorism is most effective at spreading fear when given widespread media coverage (Powell, 2011).
Most research focuses on framing and its impact on public opinion (Norris, Kern, & Jost, 2003; Powell, 2011; Ruigrok & van Attevelt, 2007).
A few studies have focused on the quantity of media coverage rather than the context.
Incident-level factors can impact the amount of media coverage (Chermak & Gruenewald, 2006; Nacos, 2002; Persson, 2004).
Perpetrator nationality impacts the amount of media coverage (Weimann and Brosius, 1991).
These works are largely focused on the pre-9/11, pre-digital media age.
These studies do not focus on perpetrator religion as a key predictor of coverage in the context of domestic terrorism.
The amount of coverage increases public awareness and signifies that the event is worthy of public attention.
Media frames matter but can only have influence if they reach an audience.
The study addresses two gaps:
Factors that explain differences in the quantity of media coverage post-9/11 and in the digital media age.
How perpetrator religion impacts these coverage disparities.
The paper is organized as follows:
Literature on media coverage of violence, crime, and terrorism.
Methodological approach, sample, and analyses.
Results, policy implications, public perception, and avenues for future research.
Media Coverage
Most of the information we get about the world outside of our local context comes from media.
Media play a vital role in how we form ideas about people, places, and things which we have not personally experienced (McCombs, 2003).
Media attention lends legitimacy to the voices and frames that are chosen to be featured (Bekkers, Beunders, Edwards, & Moody, 2011).
Media coverage also amplifies incidents and ideas by providing a platform to spread certain positions and perspectives to a broader audience (Bekkers, Beunders, Edwards, and Moody, 2011).
This platform is further expanded by members of the public disseminating media amongst themselves (Nacos, 2002).
Media coverage of subjects significantly increased online discussion of that topic immediately, and this effect persisted for nearly a week (King, Schneer, and White, 2017).
People also discuss news media content in various forums, resulting in further – not necessarily accurate – analysis of the information provided.
The rapid spread of information – regardless of its veracity – is especially common when focusing events occur.
A focusing event is a sudden, attention-grabbing event that draws public awareness to an issue (Kingdon, 1995).
Attention-grabbing and easy to politicize.
Relatively uncommon.
Reveal a cause of harm or potential harm.
Depicted as being particular to certain areas or groups (Kingdon, 1995).
When something becomes a focusing event, debates and discussions surrounding certain policy topics markedly increase and receive greater media attention (Kingdon, 1995).
Media coverage does not necessarily determine how we feel about these issues, but it sets the tone for which issues we discuss and how we discuss them (McCombs, 2003).
Media creates a perspective for viewers that may be incongruent with reality, particularly when discussing an issue that people do not directly experience (Gerbner, 1998).
Media are primarily responsible for providing information, and thus frames, to the public in the aftermath of a terrorist attack (Altheide, 1987).
There is clear evidence that media coverage impacts public perception across a host of topics including civic engagement (McCarthy, McPhail, & Smith, 1996), mental health issues (Stack, 2003), and national security threats (Slone, 2000).
Both news media (Graziano, Schuck, & Martin, 2010; Miller & Davis, 2008; Weitzer & Tuch, 2005) and entertainment media (Callanan & Rosenberger, 2011; Donahue & Miller, 2006; Donovan & Klahm, 2015; Eschholz, Blackwell, Gertz, & Chiricos, 2002; Kearns & Young, 2017) impact the public’s views of crime and justice.
Media depictions are especially impactful when people do not have direct experience with a topic (Adoni & Mane, 1984).
Media are primarily responsible for providing information to the public, who use that information to contextualize and understand terrorism.
When news media spend time on an issue, this suggests to the public that the topic is valid and important for understanding the world around them.
The amount of attention that a story gets is an indicator of its importance (McCombs, 2003).
The “CNN effect” – whereby media influence politics and government during conflict and natural disasters – suggests that media framing can impact public opinion and potentially sway policy decisions (Gilboa, 2005).
Exposure to media coverage of terrorist attacks is positively correlated to perceived personal risk for being victimized, fear of others (Nellis & Savage, 2012), and short-term anxiety levels (Slone, 2000).
Media are especially impactful at setting public discourse and, as a result, influencing public opinion in regard to limiting or protecting personal freedoms and civil liberties, as they feature and prioritize certain political viewpoints and narratives over others (Guasti & Mansfeldova, 2013; Hall, 2012; Norris, Kern, & Just, 2003).
Political organizations use media to set the priorities of the public (Chermak, 2003), which means that biases in media reporting can have real-world consequences.
In short, media coverage influences public opinion and perceptions of the world, which can, in turn, influence how the public perceives relevant people, policies, and groups.
Media Coverage of Violence
Violent crime has been declining steadily in the United States for the past twenty years.
Public perceptions of violent crime do not reflect this.
People still perceive that it is increasing as the violent crime rate in the United States decreases (Gramlich, 2017).
Media may influence this disparity in perceptions of violence.
Homicides receive a disproportionate amount of news coverage relative to both the actual risk of being victimized and the frequency of the crime (Paulsen, 2003; Peelo, Francis, Soothill, Pearson, & Ackery, 2004; Sorenson, Manz, & Berk, 1998).
Violence, broadly construed, is one of the most prominent topics in the news media, and enjoys something of a privileged position, yet it is rare in day-to-day life for much of the audience.
Media influence increases as actual experience with a problem decreases, which could explain this discrepancy between real and perceived violent crime rates (Slone, 2000).
Half of Americans are concerned that they or a family member will be the victim of a terrorist attack, despite the actual risk being minuscule (Jones & Cox, 2015).
Coverage of violence fills that role, while also potentially providing useful information to the viewer.
An event may be attention-grabbing but lose relevance quickly.
For a topic to maintain relevance, it must receive ongoing coverage by the media for approximately one to eight weeks (Coleman, McCombs, Shaw, & Weaver, 2009).
The perceived relevance of an incident fades as time passes without the media referring to it (Coleman et al., 2009).
Stories are selected for coverage based on how much attention they can potentially attract (Xiang & Sarvary, 2007). This is due to the "infotainment" format of news media.
Media Coverage of Terrorism
While some terrorist attacks are sensationalized and extensively covered, the majority receive little to no media attention (Chermak & Gruenewald, 2006).
An issue’s relevance influences the amount of media coverage that it receives (McCarthy et al., 1996).
Some terrorist attacks may be deemed more relevant than others due to their inherently political, attention-grabbing nature, and potential to be a focusing event.
Terrorism lends itself to being used as a focusing event, as it is uncommon and can raise awareness of potential weak points in national security.
Examples of terrorist attacks used as focusing events:
Dylann Roof’s terrorist attack sparked fierce debates about the Confederate Flag and gun control policy.
Robert Lewis Dear’s attack on a Planned Parenthood facility was used to argue that promoting misleading information could have deadly consequences.
These attacks are used as focusing events, shifting the public discourse to political topics secondary to terrorism itself and often facilitating or inspiring new policy.
Brian Jenkins (1974, p. 4) stated that “terrorism is theater,” a metaphor reflecting that perpetrators engage in violence to communicate with an audience.
Media coverage of attacks amplifies a group’s messaging and sensationalizes the event (Picard, 1993).
Media and terrorist groups have a mutually reinforcing relationship.
Media do not cover all terrorism equally.
Attacks received more coverage if there were casualties, if it was a hijacking, if an airline was targeted, or if domestic groups were involved (Chermak and Gruenewald, 2006).
Perpetrator identity was not considered as a factor that would impact the amount of coverage an attack receives.
Even minor attacks may receive coverage if the target, location, or groups involved are of high symbolic or political significance to the public (Nacos, 2002).
A terrorist attack will receive less coverage if it is framed as a crime (Persson, 2004).
Whether an attack is framed as terrorism or a crime is complicated by the fact that there is no one accepted definition of terrorism to rely on, even among experts (Schmid, 2013; Spaaij & Hamm, 2015).
There are myriad potential factors that can impact why a particular terrorist attack receives more news coverage than others.
The study examines how the following factors influence the amount of news coverage that a given terrorist attack will receive:
Who committed the attack
What the target was
How many people were killed
Events are more newsworthy if they can be typified as reflecting current beliefs and social structure, and can be scripted in ways that reinforce stereotypes (Lundman, 2003).
Consistent with the social identity perspective (Tajfel & Turner, 1986), media in the predominantly white, Christian United States may portray members of this in-group in a more favorable way than people who are not members of the majority race or religion.
Actors portraying terrorists are generally Muslim or Arab, while white actors play the hero (Alsultany, 2012).
Most Arab movie characters are portrayed as dangerous stereotypes (Shaheen, 2012).
Perpetrators of terrorism are disproportionately non-white (Gilliam & Iyengar, 2000).
Media coverage may explain public perceptions of terrorism and identity.
To Americans, there is an implicit association between terrorism, people of Middle Eastern descent, and Islam (Alsultany, 2012; Gottschalk & Greenberg, 2008; Park, Felix, & Lee, 2007; Saleem & Anderson, 2013).
Muslims are increasingly viewed as a national security threat (Allouche & Lind, 2010).
Members of the public are more likely to consider an attack terrorism when the perpetrator is Muslim (Huff and Kertzer, 2017).
Incidents committed by Muslims were more likely to be labeled as terrorism and were also judged more harshly (West & Lloyd, 2017).
Muslims were vastly overrepresented in broadcast media coverage of terrorism (Dixon and Williams, 2015).
News stories about Middle Eastern people often focused on terrorism, asylum seekers, and cultural practices that are alien to Western cultures (Akbarzadeh & Smith, 2005).
Media may frame terrorism as a specifically Muslim problem because that is a dominant narrative (Sultan, 2016).
Domestic terrorism is often portrayed as a minor threat committed by mentally ill perpetrators, whereas terrorism influenced by radical interpretation of Islam is framed as a hostile outside force (Powell, 2011).
If the perpetrators were Muslim and the victims Christian, the innocence and goodness of the victims and their spirituality will often be presented in juxtaposition with Islam (Powell, 2011).
When the perpetrator(s) of a terrorist attack are members of an out-group or “other,” we should expect to see more media coverage.
Hypothesis 1
Terrorist attacks will receive more media coverage when the perpetrator is Muslim than when the perpetrator is not Muslim.
Hypothesis 2
Terrorist attacks will receive more media coverage when the perpetrator is arrested than the perpetrator is not arrested.
Hypothesis 3
Terrorist attacks will receive more media coverage when the target is a governmental facility or employee(s) than when the target is non-governmental.
Hypothesis 4
Terrorist attacks will receive increased media coverage as the number of fatalities caused by the attack increases.
Alternative explanations
White homicide victims receive more media coverage than minority victims (Gruenewald, Chermak, & Pizarro, 2013).
Attacks against an out-group receive less media coverage.
Certain dates, such as Hitler’s birthday and the anniversary of 9/11, attract more violence.
When attacks occur within close proximity to these symbolic dates, they may receive more media coverage.
We may expect to see less media coverage when responsibility for the attack is unknown (Weimann & Brosius, 1991; Weimann & Winn, 1994).
We may expect to see more coverage when the individual(s) responsible are connected with a larger group that uses terrorism.
Classification differences can explain variation in coverage, potentially resulting in ambiguous cases receiving less media attention.
Methods
Data
The data for this study consisted of media coverage for terrorist attacks in the United States between 2006 and 2015.
Listed in the GTD.
The GTD lists 170 terrorist attacks during this ten-year span.
136 terrorism episodes in the United States during this time.
Two sources: LexisNexis Academic and CNN.com.
To measure media coverage.
LexisNexis Academic searches through the full text of thousands of news publications.
Limited the search results to newspaper coverage from US-based sources between the date of the attack and the end of 2016.
Searches news articles from national sources.
Searched CNN.com’s archives to obtain additional news coverage that is solely in digital format.
3541 news articles in the dataset.
Variables:
Dependent variable:
The outcome variable for all hypotheses was the number of news stories about the incident.
Added the number of relevant articles from LexisNexis Academic and CNN.com to yield the total number of articles for each terrorism series.
Estimate models with the total number of articles from major sources only (35.6% of the articles) and with the total number of articles from other sources only (64.4% of the articles).
Independent variables:
Three binary perpetrator-level variables were coded: perpetrator Muslim, perpetrator arrested, and unknown perpetrator.
Three binary target type variables were coded: law enforcement/governmental target, Muslim target, and minority target.
Fatalities as the number of people killed – excluding the perpetrator(s) – in each terrorism series.
Included a binary indicator to denote whether or not the attack occurred near a symbolically significant event in the United States as a control.
Results:
Negative binomial regression models are most appropriate since the dependent variable is a non-negative count of news articles per attack.
Attacks by Muslims receive significantly more coverage than attacks by non-Muslims.
If the perpetrator is Muslim, we see 357% more news stories about the attack.
357% more news stories.
287% increase in coverage when the perpetrator is arrested.
211% increase if the target is governmental.
46% increase per fatality, on average.
Discussion
The study examined quantitative differences in media coverage of terrorist attacks.
The quantity of articles is important for public perception of terrorism.
This study is the first post-9/11 and digital media age study focused on the quantity of coverage that terrorist attacks receive.
This is the first study to explicitly examine how perpetrator religion impacts coverage across such a wide range of terrorism cases.
By modeling coverage over all terrorist attacks in the United States during a ten-year period, we are able to identify trends in coverage.
Perpetrator religion matters for the quantity of coverage that an attack receives.
attacks perpetrated by Muslims receive drastically more media coverage than attacks by non-Muslims.
This finding is consistent with the literature on social identity (Tajfel & Turner, 1986) that highlights in-group and out-group dynamics whereby people who are perceived as “others” are portrayed and perceived more negatively.
Media find bias against Muslims and Arabs in the context of entertainment media (Shaheen, 2012).
Similar biases against Muslims exist in media coverage of terrorism.
This may explain why people implicitly connect terrorism and Islam and view Muslims as a threat to national security (Allouche & Lind, 2010).
Each of the other hypotheses was supported.
When a perpetrator of an attack is arrested we find significantly more coverage.
Attacks against the government receive more coverage.
The number of fatalities in a given attack has a significant impact on the extent of coverage.
Models, the variables testing other counterarguments were not significant.
Attacks that targeted either Muslims specifically or minorities in general did not receive less media coverage.
Incidents that occurred near significant dates did not receive more coverage.
There was no difference in the amount of coverage for attacks connected to a larger group versus those without this connection.
The results and the robustness of the models demonstrate that media give disproportionate coverage to terrorism when the perpetrator is Muslim, though other factors also matter.
The identity of a perpetrator as Muslim has primacy as the key driver of the amount of coverage, relative to each of the other factors.
Limitations and Future Directions
Future research could replicate the project with broadcast coverage.
Conduct similar analyses in other countries to address concerns with generalizability.
Some media outlets may selectively cover certain attacks more than others in a way that reflects the ideological perspective of the news organization.
Policy Implications
The media does not cover some terrorist attacks enough.
Attacks by Muslim perpetrators received less coverage is unsubstantiated.
Media do not cover these events equally.
Attacks perpetrated by Muslim receive a disproportionate amount of media coverage.
Whether the disproportionate coverage is a conscious decision on the part of journalists or not, this stereotyping reinforces cultural narratives about what and who should be feared.
These findings help explain why half of Americans fear that they or someone they know will be a victim of terrorism and implicitly link terrorism and Islam (Saleem & Anderson, 2013).
Reality demonstrates, however, that these fears are misplaced.
One way to combat misplaced fears about terrorism is to change the public narrative on terrorism to cover attacks more evenly and based on consistently applied criteria.