Chapter 17: Rotter & Mischel – Cognitive Social Learning Theory
Overview of Cognitive Social Learning Theory
Integrates behaviorist learning principles (e.g., reinforcement) with cognitive variables (e.g., expectancies, goals, values)
Central assumption: People interpret, anticipate, and symbolically process events; these cognitions interact with environmental contingencies to guide behavior
Rejects Skinnerian notion that immediate, external reinforcement solely shapes behavior; instead, future expectations and subjective meanings are prime determinants
Two principal architects
Julian B. Rotter – interactionist, quantitative formulas for predicting behavior
Walter Mischel – emphasis on person–situation interaction and cognitive-affective processing
Biography of Julian Rotter
Born Oct 22 1916, Brooklyn; third (youngest) son of Jewish immigrants
Great Depression → family financial decline → concern for social injustice & situational effects on behavior
Early exposure: Adler, Freud, Menninger found on library shelf; attended Adler’s lectures
B.S. Brooklyn College (1937, chemistry major but psychology credits > chemistry)
M.A. Univ. of Iowa (1938); Ph.D. Indiana Univ. (1941); internship Worcester State Hospital (met wife Clara)
Army psychologist WWII (>3 yrs)
Faculty: Ohio State (with George Kelly) → Univ. of Connecticut (1963–87)
Major works: Social Learning & Clinical Psychology (1954); Clinical Psychology (1964); Rotter I-E Scale (1966); Interpersonal Trust Scale (1967)
Honors: APA Distinguished Scientific Contribution (1988); Distinguished Contribution to Clinical Training (1989)
Rotter’s Social Learning Theory: Five Basic Hypotheses
Humans interact with meaningful environments; subjective meaning mediates stimulus→response
Personality is learned and modifiable throughout life
Personality possesses basic unity via relatively stable evaluations of experiences
Motivation is goal-directed; behavior serves movement toward goals rather than tension reduction
Humans anticipate events; perceived progress toward anticipated goals is used to evaluate reinforcers (Empirical Law of Effect)
Predicting Specific Behaviors
Four immediate variables
Behavior Potential (BP) – likelihood of a specific response in a given situation
Expectancy (E) – subjective probability that a behavior will produce a given reinforcement
Reinforcement Value (RV) – preference for one reinforcement vs. others when probabilities equal
Psychological Situation (s) – total of internal & external cues as perceived at a moment
Basic Prediction Formula BP{x1,s1,ra} = f\left(E{x1,ra,s1} + RV{a,s1}\right)
Example: Likelihood La Juan rests head in boring lecture depends on expectancy that sleeping will occur plus value of sleep under those cues
Predicting General Behaviors
Generalized Expectancies (GE) – mean expectancies built from prior analogous experiences
Needs – categories of goal-directed behavior; semantic shift: focus on person (need) vs. environment (goal)
Six broad need categories with illustrative behaviors
Recognition-Status – e.g., excelling in bridge for praise
Dominance – persuading colleagues, directing others
Independence – declining help to repair bicycle
Protection-Dependency – asking spouse to care for you when ill
Love & Affection – doing favors to elicit warmth
Physical Comfort – turning on AC, hugging
Need complex components
Need Potential (NP) – aggregate BP of behaviors that satisfy a need
Freedom of Movement (FM) – average expectancy of success for behaviors within need area
Need Value (NV) – average reinforcement value of outcomes within need area
General Prediction Formula
NP = f(FM + NV)
Internal–External Control of Reinforcement (Locus of Control)
Core idea: people differ in generalized expectancy that their actions control outcomes
I-E Scale (Rotter 1966) – 23 scored forced-choice items; high = external
Misconceptions clarified
Scores indicate expectancy, not direct causes of behavior
Scale measures generalized, not situation-specific control
Internality–Externality is a continuum, not dichotomy
Extreme internal or external scores may be maladaptive
Interpersonal Trust Scale
Interpersonal trust = expectancy that another’s word or promise is reliable
25 scored items, 5-point agree–disagree scale; high score = high trust
High-trust individuals: less likely to lie/cheat, more respectful & popular, not more gullible
Societal functioning relies on moderate–high interpersonal trust (food safety, mail, aviation, etc.)
Maladaptive Behavior (Rotter)
Persistent actions that fail to move person toward goals
Often produced by
High need value + low freedom of movement (unrealistic goals)
Inadequate skills/information → low FM
Faulty situation evaluation → underestimated abilities
Over-generalization from one area of failure to others
Psychotherapy (Rotter)
Therapist = active problem-solving teacher within social interaction
Goals: harmonize FM and NV; reduce avoidance & defense
Two main strategies
Changing Goals
Resolve conflicts between competing needs (e.g., adolescent independence vs. dependency)
Redirect from destructive or overly lofty goals using reinforcement & education
Eliminating Low Expectancies
Teach new skills/information (assertiveness, interpersonal techniques)
Correct faulty evaluations of current situations
Counter maladaptive generalizations by reinforcing small successes
Techniques: modeling, extinction of inappropriate behavior, perspective-taking, encouraging observational period (“quiet observer” experiment)
Biography of Walter Mischel
Born Feb 22 1930, Vienna; Jewish family fled 1938 Nazi annexation → Brooklyn upbringing
NYU undergrad; influenced by art, Freud, existentialists; MA City College of NY (clinical); social work in slums → skepticism of psychoanalysis & emphasis on empirical validation
Ph.D. Ohio State (1956) under Rotter & influenced by George Kelly
Fieldwork in Caribbean on spirit possession & delay of gratification
Academic posts: Colorado, Harvard (with Allport), Stanford (with Bandura), Columbia Univ.
Seminal book: Personality and Assessment (1968) – critique of trait prediction
Honors: APA Distinguished Scientific Contribution (1982)
Background to Mischel’s Theory
Initial stance against broad, decontextualized traits → “Consistency Paradox” (people seen as consistent, data show variability)
Shift to reconciliation: stable traits and situation-linked variability coexist
Consistency Paradox
Lay & professional belief in cross-situational trait consistency (e.g., honesty) vs. empirical correlations ≈ 0.30
Hartshorne & May (1928) – honesty varied across test, game, party situations
Person–Situation Interaction
Behavior = function of personal dispositions + specific situation cues
Same person may act shy with women, bold with men; classification requires context
Cognitive-Affective Personality System (CAPS)
Proposed by Mischel & Shoda to solve paradox
Each person shows a stable behavioral signature: “If A → X; if B → Y”
Example: If provoked by wife ⇒ aggression; if provoked by boss ⇒ submission
Stability lies in the pattern of variability, not in invariant behaviors
Behavior Prediction Statement
“If personality is a stable system that processes information about situations, then as situations vary, behavior will vary in a predictable pattern.”
Situation Variables
When environmental cues are powerful & uniform (e.g., moving film), situation dominates
When personal meanings diverge (e.g., job lay-off), person variables dominate
Cognitive-Affective Units (5 classes)
Encoding Strategies & Personal Constructs
Individual ways of categorizing stimuli; selective attention, subjective interpretation
Competencies & Self-Regulatory Strategies
Abilities, problem-solving skills, self-imposed goals, self-reinforcement
Expectancies & Beliefs
Behavior-Outcome ("If I study, then I’ll excel")
Stimulus-Outcome (lightning → thunder) – basis of classical conditioning
Goals & Values
Desired outcomes steer attention & planning; emotionally charged → stability
Affective Responses
Emotions, physiological reactions inseparable from cognitions; color all other units
Empirical Illustrations
Delay of Gratification (Mischel & Ebbesen 1970): children who distracted themselves (altered encoding) could wait longer → cognitive transformation modulates behavior
Locus of Control & Holocaust Heroes (Midlarsky et al. 2005)
Sample: 80 rescuers vs. 73 bystanders vs. 43 pre-war immigrants
Higher internal control associated with autonomy, risk-taking, empathy, social responsibility
Personality variables correctly classified hero status 93\% of cases
If–Then Predictions of Warmth (Kammrath et al. 2005)
Participants inferred differing warmth of a “kiss-up” toward professors vs. peers → lay understanding of behavioral signatures
Conditional Self-Evaluations & Emotion (Mendoza-Denton et al. 2001)
“I am good when I work hard” buffers sadness after imagined failures compared to unconditional “I am a failure”
Critique of Cognitive Social Learning Theory
Research Support: extensive (esp. Rotter’s I-E scale; growing CAPS studies)
Testability/Falsifiability: Moderate–high; formulas abstract but constructs measurable; CAPS yields testable if-then hypotheses
Organization: Offers unifying framework linking learning, cognition, affect, and traits
Practicality: Provides guidance for therapy (goal restructuring, skill training) and assessment (locus of control, behavioral signatures)
Internal Consistency: Generally high; clear definitions; if-then logic coherent
Parsimony: Relatively parsimonious given complexity of human behavior
Concept of Humanity (Rotter & Mischel)
Determinism vs. Free Will: Interactionist; past learning + current cognitions, yet people set goals and select strategies (some choice)
Nature vs. Nurture: Heavy emphasis on social learning; Mischel acknowledges genetic predispositions that influence CAPS patterns
Past vs. Future: Forward-looking; anticipations of reinforcement and goals dominate
Conscious vs. Unconscious: Lean conscious (explicit expectancies, plans), but recognize implicit affect & mis-evaluations
Uniqueness vs. Universality: Rotter – moderate; formulas assume common variables. Mischel – uniqueness via individual behavioral signatures.
Optimism vs. Pessimism: Guarded optimism; behavior change possible through learning & cognitive restructuring
Key Formulas & Definitions (Quick Reference)
Empirical Law of Effect: Reinforcement = any event moving person toward a goal
Basic Prediction Formula: BP = f(E + RV)
General Prediction Formula: NP = f(FM + NV)
Locus of Control: Generalized expectancy regarding control of reinforcement; Internal ↔ External continuum
Interpersonal Trust: Expectancy that others’ communications are reliable
Behavioral Signature: Stable if-then pattern of variability reflecting CAPS