Empiricism: The idea that knowledge comes from experience, and that science should rely on observation and experimentation.
Structuralism: An early school of psychology that used introspection to explore the structure of the human mind; associated with Edward Titchener.
Functionalism: An early school of psychology focused on how mental and behavioral processes functionâhow they enable organisms to adapt, survive, and flourish; linked to William James.
Experimental Psychology: The study of behavior and thinking using the experimental method.
Behaviorism: The view that psychology should be an objective science that studies behavior without reference to mental processes.
Humanistic Psychology: A perspective that emphasized human growth potential and the importance of having our needs for love and acceptance satisfied.
Cognitive Neuroscience: The interdisciplinary study of brain activity linked with cognition, including perception, thinking, memory, and language.
Psychology: The scientific study of behavior and mental processes.
NatureâNurture Issue: The debate over the relative contributions of biology (nature) and experience (nurture) to the development of psychological traits and behaviors.
Natural Selection: The principle that traits contributing to reproduction and survival are most likely to be passed on to future generations.
Levels of Analysis: The different complementary viewsâfrom biological to psychological to social-culturalâfor analyzing any phenomenon.
Biopsychosocial Approach: An integrated approach that incorporates biological, psychological, and social-cultural levels of analysis.
Behavioral Psychology: The scientific study of observable behavior and its explanation by principles of learning.
Biological Psychology: The scientific study of the links between biological (genetic, neural, hormonal) and psychological processes.
Cognitive Psychology: The scientific study of mental activities such as thinking, knowing, remembering, and communicating.
Evolutionary Psychology: The study of the evolution of behavior and mind using principles of natural selection.
Psychodynamic Psychology: The study of how unconscious drives and conflicts influence behavior, used in treating psychological disorders.
Social-Cultural Psychology: The study of how situations and cultures affect behavior and thinking.
Psychometrics: The scientific study of the measurement of human abilities, attitudes, and traits.
Basic Research: Pure science that aims to increase the scientific knowledge base.
Developmental Psychology: The study of physical, cognitive, and social change throughout the life span.
Educational Psychology: The study of how psychological processes affect and can enhance teaching and learning.
Personality Psychology: The study of an individualâs characteristic pattern of thinking, feeling, and acting.
Social Psychology: The scientific study of how we think about, influence, and relate to one another.
Applied Research: Scientific study that aims to solve practical problems.
Industrial-Organizational (I/O) Psychology: The application of psychological concepts to optimizing human behavior in workplaces.
Human Factors Psychology: A subfield of I/O psychology that explores how people and machines interact and how to make environments safe and user-friendly.
Counseling Psychology: A branch of psychology that assists people with life problems and helps them achieve well-being.
Clinical Psychology: A branch of psychology that studies, assesses, and treats people with psychological disorders.
Psychiatry: A branch of medicine dealing with psychological disorders; practiced by physicians who can prescribe medication and offer therapy.
Positive Psychology: The scientific study of human flourishing, with the goal of promoting strengths and virtues that help individuals and communities thrive.
Community Psychology: The study of how people interact with their environments and how social institutions affect individuals and groups.
Testing Effect: Improved memory after retrieving information, rather than simply rereading it.
SQ3R: A study method involving five steps: Survey, Question, Read, Retrieve, Review.
Wilhelm Wundt (p. 3): Known as the âfather of psychology,â he established the first psychology laboratory in Germany in 1879 and focused on studying consciousness through introspection.
G. Stanley Hall (p. 3): A student of Wundt who established the first psychology lab in the U.S. and founded the American Psychological Association (APA), serving as its first president.
William James (p. 4): A philosopher-psychologist who founded functionalism and wrote The Principles of Psychology, one of the first psychology textbooks.
Mary Whiton Calkins (p. 4): A pioneering memory researcher and the first woman president of the APA; she was denied a Harvard degree despite completing all the requirements.
Margaret Floy Washburn (p. 5): The first woman to earn a Ph.D. in psychology; known for her work in animal behavior and motor theory development.
Sigmund Freud (p. 5): The founder of psychoanalysis, a theory of personality and therapy focusing on unconscious motives and childhood experiences.
John B. Watson (p. 6): Founder of behaviorism; famously conducted the âLittle Albertâ experiment demonstrating conditioned emotional responses.
B. F. Skinner (p. 6): A leading behaviorist who developed the theory of operant conditioning using reinforcement and punishment to shape behavior.
Carl Rogers (p. 6): A humanistic psychologist who emphasized the importance of unconditional positive regard, empathy, and the client-centered approach to therapy.
Ivan Pavlov (p. 9): A Russian physiologist known for discovering classical conditioning through his experiments with dogs and salivation.
Jean Piaget (p. 9): A developmental psychologist known for his theory of cognitive development in children.
Charles Darwin (p. 10): Naturalist who developed the theory of evolution by natural selection; his ideas inspired evolutionary psychology.
Dorothea Dix (p. 14): A reformer who advocated for the humane treatment of the mentally ill and helped establish mental hospitals in the U.S.
Hindsight Bias (p. 31): The tendency to believe, after learning an outcome, that one would have foreseen it ("I-knew-it-all-along" phenomenon).
Critical Thinking (p. 35): Thinking that does not blindly accept arguments and conclusions; it examines assumptions, evaluates evidence, and assesses conclusions.
Theory (p. 38): An explanation using an integrated set of principles that organizes observations and predicts behaviors or events.
Hypothesis (p. 38): A testable prediction, often implied by a theory.
Operational Definition (p. 39): A carefully worded statement of the exact procedures (operations) used in a research study.
Replication (p. 39): Repeating the essence of a research study, usually with different participants in different situations, to see if the findings extend to other circumstances.
Case Study (p. 40): A descriptive technique in which one individual or group is studied in depth to reveal universal principles.
Naturalistic Observation (p. 40): Observing and recording behavior in naturally occurring situations without trying to manipulate or control the situation.
Survey (p. 42): A technique for ascertaining the self-reported attitudes or behaviors of a particular group, usually by questioning a representative sample.
Sampling Bias (p. 43): A flawed sampling process that produces an unrepresentative sample.
Population (p. 43): All those in a group being studied, from which samples may be drawn.
Random Sample (p. 43): A sample that fairly represents a population because each member has an equal chance of inclusion.
Correlation (p. 46): A measure of the extent to which two factors vary together and how well one predicts the other.
Correlation Coefficient (p. 46): A statistical index of the relationship between two things (from â1.0 to +1.0).
Scatterplot (p. 46): A graphed cluster of dots that represents the values of two variables; the slope suggests the direction of the relationship.
Illusory Correlation (p. 50): The perception of a relationship where none exists.
Experiment (p. 51): A research method in which an investigator manipulates one or more factors (independent variables) to observe the effect on a dependent variable.
Experimental Group (p. 51): The group exposed to the treatment in an experiment.
Control Group (p. 51): The group not exposed to the treatment; serves as a comparison for evaluating the effect of the treatment.
Random Assignment (p. 51): Assigning participants to experimental and control groups by chance, minimizing preexisting differences.
Double-Blind Procedure (p. 51): An experimental procedure in which both participants and research staff are ignorant about whether participants received the treatment or a placebo.
Placebo Effect (p. 52): Experimental results caused by expectations alone; any effect on behavior caused by the administration of an inert substance or condition.
Independent Variable (p. 52): The factor that is manipulated; the variable whose effect is being studied.
Confounding Variable (p. 52): A factor other than the independent variable that might produce an effect in an experiment.
Dependent Variable (p. 52): The outcome that is measured; the variable that may change when the independent variable is manipulated.
Validity (p. 53): The extent to which a test or experiment measures or predicts what it is supposed to.
Descriptive Statistics (p. 57): Numerical data used to measure and describe characteristics of groups, including measures of central tendency and variation.
Mode (p. 57): The most frequently occurring score in a distribution.
Mean (p. 57): The arithmetic average of a distribution, obtained by adding the scores and dividing by the number of scores.
Median (p. 57): The middle score in a distribution; half the scores are above it and half are below.
Skewed Distribution (p. 58): A representation of scores that lack symmetry around their average value.
Range (p. 58): The difference between the highest and lowest scores in a distribution.
Standard Deviation (p. 58): A computed measure of how much scores vary around the mean score.
Normal Curve (p. 59): A symmetrical, bell-shaped curve that describes the distribution of many types of data.
Inferential Statistics (p. 60): Numerical data that allow one to generalizeâto infer from sample data the probability of something being true of a population.
Statistical Significance (p. 60): A statistical statement of how likely it is that an obtained result occurred by chance.
Culture (p. 65): The enduring behaviors, ideas, attitudes, values, and traditions shared by a group of people and transmitted from one generation to the next.
Informed Consent (p. 68): Giving potential participants enough information about a study to enable them to choose whether they wish to participate.
Debriefing (p. 68): The post-experimental explanation of a study, including its purpose and any deceptions, to its participants.
Biological Psychology (p. 77): The scientific study of the links between biological (genetic, neural, hormonal) and psychological processes.
Neuron (p. 78): A nerve cell; the basic building block of the nervous system.
Dendrites (p. 78): Branching extensions of a neuron that receive messages and conduct impulses toward the cell body.
Axon (p. 78): The neuron extension that passes messages through its branches to other neurons or to muscles or glands.
Myelin Sheath (p. 78): A fatty tissue layer segmentally encasing the axons of some neurons; enables faster transmission of impulses.
Action Potential (p. 78): A neural impulse; a brief electrical charge that travels down an axon.
Refractory Period (p. 79): A period of inactivity after a neuron has fired.
Threshold (p. 80): The level of stimulation required to trigger a neural impulse.
All-or-None Response (p. 80): A neuron's reaction of either firing (with a full-strength response) or not firing at all.
Synapse (p. 80): The junction between the axon tip of the sending neuron and the dendrite or cell body of the receiving neuron.
Neurotransmitters (p. 80): Chemical messengers that cross the synaptic gaps between neurons.
Reuptake (p. 80): A neurotransmitter's reabsorption by the sending neuron.
Endorphins (p. 82): Natural, opiate-like neurotransmitters linked to pain control and pleasure.
Agonist (p. 82): A molecule that increases a neurotransmitter's action.
Antagonist (p. 83): A molecule that inhibits or blocks a neurotransmitter's action.
Nervous System (p. 86): The bodyâs speedy, electrochemical communication network, consisting of all the nerve cells of the peripheral and central nervous systems.
Central Nervous System (CNS) (p. 86): The brain and spinal cord.
Peripheral Nervous System (PNS) (p. 86): The sensory and motor neurons that connect the CNS to the rest of the body.
Nerves (p. 86): Bundled axons that form neural âcablesâ connecting the CNS with muscles, glands, and sense organs.
Sensory (Afferent) Neurons (p. 86): Neurons that carry incoming information from the sensory receptors to the CNS.
Motor (Efferent) Neurons (p. 86): Neurons that carry outgoing information from the CNS to the muscles and glands.
Interneurons (p. 87): Neurons within the CNS that communicate internally and intervene between sensory inputs and motor outputs.
Somatic Nervous System (p. 87): The part of the PNS that controls the bodyâs skeletal muscles.
Autonomic Nervous System (ANS) (p. 87): The part of the PNS that controls the glands and the muscles of the internal organs.
Sympathetic Nervous System (p. 87): Arouses the body, mobilizing its energy in stressful situations.
Parasympathetic Nervous System (p. 87): Calms the body, conserving its energy.
Reflex (p. 89): A simple, automatic response to a sensory stimulus.
Endocrine System (p. 90): The bodyâs âslowâ chemical communication system; a set of glands that secrete hormones into the bloodstream.
Hormones (p. 90): Chemical messengers that are manufactured by the endocrine glands and travel through the bloodstream.
Adrenal Glands (p. 91): Glands that help arouse the body in times of stress by releasing epinephrine and norepinephrine.
Pituitary Gland (p. 91): The endocrine systemâs most influential gland; regulates growth and controls other endocrine glands.
Lesion (p. 94): Tissue destruction; a brain lesion is a naturally or experimentally caused destruction of brain tissue.
Electroencephalogram (EEG) (p. 95): An amplified recording of the waves of electrical activity sweeping across the brainâs surface.
CT (Computed Tomography) Scan (p. 95): A series of X-ray photographs taken from different angles to show brain structure.
PET (Positron Emission Tomography) Scan (p. 95): A visual display of brain activity that detects where a radioactive form of glucose goes during a task.
MRI (Magnetic Resonance Imaging) (p. 95): A technique that uses magnetic fields and radio waves to produce images of brain anatomy.
fMRI (Functional MRI) (p. 96): Reveals brain function as well as structure by comparing successive MRI scans.
Brainstem (p. 97): The oldest part and central core of the brain, responsible for automatic survival functions.
Medulla (p. 97): The base of the brainstem; controls heartbeat and breathing.
Thalamus (p. 97): The brainâs sensory control center; it directs messages to the sensory receiving areas in the cortex.
Reticular Formation (p. 98): A nerve network that plays an important role in controlling arousal.
Cerebellum (p. 98): The âlittle brainâ at the rear of the brainstem; coordinates movement and balance.
Limbic System (p. 98): Neural system associated with emotions and drives.
Amygdala (p. 99): Two lima-bean-sized neural clusters in the limbic system linked to emotion.
Hypothalamus (p. 99): A neural structure lying below the thalamus; it directs maintenance activities and helps govern the endocrine system.
Cerebral Cortex (p. 104): The intricate fabric of interconnected neural cells covering the cerebral hemispheres; the bodyâs ultimate control and processing center.
Glial Cells (p. 104): Cells in the nervous system that support, nourish, and protect neurons.
Frontal Lobes (p. 105): Involved in speaking, muscle movements, and making plans and judgments.
Parietal Lobes (p. 105): Receives sensory input for touch and body position.
Occipital Lobes (p. 105): Includes areas that receive information from the visual fields.
Temporal Lobes (p. 105): Includes the auditory areas, each receiving information from the opposite ear.
Motor Cortex (p. 105): Controls voluntary movements.
Somatosensory Cortex (p. 107): Registers and processes body touch and movement sensations.
Association Areas (p. 109): Areas involved in higher mental functions such as learning, remembering, thinking, and speaking.
Plasticity (p. 111): The brainâs ability to change, especially during childhood, by reorganizing after damage or by building new pathways.
Neurogenesis (p. 112): The formation of new neurons.
Corpus Callosum (p. 114): The large band of neural fibers connecting the two brain hemispheres.
Split Brain (p. 114): A condition resulting from surgery that isolates the brainâs two hemispheres.
Consciousness (p. 118): Our awareness of ourselves and our environment.
Cognitive Neuroscience (p. 119): The interdisciplinary study of the brain activity linked with cognition.
Dual Processing (p. 120): The principle that information is often simultaneously processed on separate conscious and unconscious tracks.
Behavior Genetics (p. 124): The study of the relative power and limits of genetic and environmental influences on behavior.
Environment (p. 124): Every external influence, from prenatal nutrition to social support.
Chromosomes (p. 124): Threadlike structures made of DNA molecules that contain the genes.
DNA (Deoxyribonucleic Acid) (p. 124): A complex molecule containing the genetic information that makes up chromosomes.
Genes (p. 124): The biochemical units of heredity that make up chromosomes.
Genome (p. 124): The complete instructions for making an organism, consisting of all the genetic material in its chromosomes.
Identical Twins (p. 125): Twins who develop from a single fertilized egg that splits in two, creating two genetically identical organisms.
Fraternal Twins (p. 125): Twins who develop from separate fertilized eggs; genetically no closer than siblings.
Molecular Genetics (p. 129): The subfield of biology that studies the molecular structure and function of genes.
Heritability (p. 129): The proportion of variation among individuals that we can attribute to genes.
Interaction (p. 131): The interplay that occurs when the effect of one factor (such as environment) depends on another factor (such as heredity).
Epigenetics (p. 131): The study of environmental influences on gene expression that occur without a DNA change.
Evolutionary Psychology (p. 135): The study of the evolution of behavior and the mind using principles of natural selection.
Natural Selection (p. 135): The principle that inherited traits that better enable survival and reproduction are more likely to be passed on.
Mutation (p. 136): A random error in gene replication that leads to a change.
Sensation (p. 152): Process of sensing stimuli from environment through sensory organs
Perception (p. 152): Organizing and interpreting sensory information to understand it
Bottom-up Processing (p. 152): Processing sensory info starting at the sensory receptors
Top-down Processing (p. 152): Using experience and expectations to interpret sensory info
Selective Attention (p. 152): Focusing on a specific stimulus while ignoring others
Inattentional Blindness (p. 154): Missing visible objects because attention is elsewhere
Change Blindness (p. 154): Failing to notice changes in the environment
Transduction (p. 155): Converting sensory input into neural signals
Psychophysics (p. 155): Study of stimulus properties and perception
Absolute Threshold (p. 156): Minimum stimulus intensity detectable 50% of the time
Signal Detection Theory (p. 156): Predicts when a stimulus is detected amid noise
Subliminal (p. 157): Below the threshold of conscious awareness
Priming (p. 157): Exposure influencing response to later stimuli unconsciously
Difference Threshold (p. 158): Smallest detectable difference between two stimuli
Weberâs Law (p. 158): Difference threshold is a constant percentage, not fixed amount
Sensory Adaptation (p. 159): Reduced sensitivity to constant stimulus over time
Perceptual Set (p. 163): Mental predisposition to perceive one thing over another
Extrasensory Perception (ESP) (p. 167): Perceiving without sensory input (e.g., telepathy)
Parapsychology (p. 167): Study of paranormal phenomena like ESP
Wavelength (p. 171): Distance between peaks of light or sound waves
Hue (p. 172): Color determined by wavelength
Intensity (p. 172): Amount of energy in a wave; brightness or loudness
Pupil (p. 172): Opening that lets light into the eye
Iris (p. 172): Colored muscle controlling pupil size
Lens (p. 172): Focuses light on retina by changing shape
Retina (p. 172): Light-sensitive layer with rods and cones
Accommodation (p. 172): Lens changing shape to focus images
Rods (p. 173): Detect black, white, gray; function in low light
Cones (p. 173): Detect color; function in daylight
Optic Nerve (p. 173): Carries visual info to the brain
Blind Spot (p. 173): No receptors where optic nerve exits eye
Fovea (p. 173): Central point in retina with dense cones for sharp vision
Feature Detectors (p. 175): Brain cells responding to specific stimulus features (edges, lines)
Parallel Processing (p. 176): Brain processing multiple aspects of stimuli simultaneously
Young-Helmholtz Trichromatic Theory (p. 178): Retina has three color receptors (red, green, blue)
Opponent-Process Theory (p. 179): Color perception based on opposing retinal processes
Gestalt (p. 182): Brainâs tendency to integrate pieces into meaningful wholes
Figure-Ground (p. 183): Distinguishing objects (figure) from background (ground)
Grouping (p. 183): Organizing stimuli into coherent groups
Depth Perception (p. 184): Ability to see objects in 3D and judge distance
Visual Cliff (p. 184): Lab device testing depth perception in infants and animals
Binocular Cues (p. 184): Depth cues from both eyes working together
Retinal Disparity (p. 184): Slight difference between images from each eye, helps judge depth
Monocular Cues (p. 185): Depth cues available to one eye alone
Phi Phenomenon (p. 185): Illusion of movement created by flashing images in rapid succession
Perceptual Constancy (p. 186): Recognizing objects as unchanging despite sensory changes
Color Constancy (p. 187): Perceiving colors as stable under varying illumination
Perceptual Adaptation (p. 191): Ability to adjust to changed sensory input
Audition (p. 194): The sense or act of hearing
Frequency (p. 195): Number of wave cycles per second; determines pitch
Pitch (p. 195): Perception of sound frequency as high or low
Middle Ear (p. 195): Contains ossicles that amplify sound vibrations
Cochlea (p. 195): Spiral-shaped, fluid-filled inner ear structure that triggers neural impulses
Inner Ear (p. 195): Contains cochlea and vestibular organs
Sensorineural Hearing Loss (p. 197): Hearing loss from damage to cochlea or auditory nerve
Conduction Hearing Loss (p. 197): Hearing loss caused by damage to mechanical system conducting sound
Cochlear Implant (p. 198): Device that converts sounds into electrical signals for auditory nerve
Place Theory (p. 199): Pitch perception depends on where cochleaâs membrane is stimulated
Frequency Theory (p. 199): Pitch perception depends on the frequency of nerve impulses
Gate-Control Theory (p. 203): Pain signals pass through âgatesâ in spinal cord that can block or allow pain
Kinesthesia (p. 209): Sense of body position and movement of limbs
Vestibular Sense (p. 209): Sense of bodyâs balance and movement in space
Sensory Interaction (p. 210): One sense influencing another (e.g., smell influencing taste)
Embodied Cognition (p. 211): Influence of bodily sensations and actions on cognitive preferences and judgments