Cognition 2: Exam Practice Questions – Language

Language

Lexical Bias Effect

  • Definition: The lexical bias effect is the phenomenon where speech errors are more likely to result in real words rather than non-words.

  • Significance: This suggests a cognitive preference for lexically valid outputs in speech production.

  • Experimental Study:

    • SLIP (Spoonerisms of Laboratory-Induced Predisposition) Technique: Participants read pairs of words silently and then aloud, designed to induce phoneme transpositions.

    • Example: "cool tart" leading to the error "tool cart."

    • Observation: Real-word errors occur more frequently than non-word errors.

    • Implication: Internal speech processing involves monitoring or feedback loops favoring lexically legitimate outputs.

    • Theoretical Support: Supports interactive processing theories, where lexical-level information influences phoneme-level selection.

Theories Explaining Lexical Bias Effect

  • Self-Monitoring Theory:

    • Speakers have an internal mechanism to evaluate speech before articulation.

    • Non-word errors are more likely to be intercepted and corrected internally.

  • Dell’s Interactive Activation Model:

    • Speech production is a multi-layered system with bidirectional activation between semantic, lexical, and phonological levels.

    • Real-word combinations receive stronger activation due to feedback.

  • Semantic-Phonological Feedback Accounts:

    • Prioritizes real-word outputs due to reinforcing interaction between word meanings and sound structures.

    • Real-word errors are more resilient and likely to be articulated.

  • Evaluation of Theories: Dell’s model is better supported by neuropsychological and experimental data.

Infant Phoneme Discrimination

  • Evidence: Infants can discriminate phonemes from birth.

  • High-Amplitude Sucking (HAS) Technique:

    • Measures changes in an infant’s sucking rate in response to auditory stimuli; an increased sucking rate indicates perception of change.

    • Study Example: Eimas et al. (1971) showed that one-month-old infants could differentiate between /ba/ and /pa/ based on voice onset time (VOT).

  • Prenatal Studies:

    • Fetal heart rate deceleration and MEG studies show fetuses (around 28 weeks gestation) can detect changes in auditory stimuli, especially prosodic features.

  • Native Language Preference: Newborns prefer their native language, suggesting phonological learning begins before birth.

  • Theoretical Alignment:

    • Supports nativist theories (Chomsky, Pinker).

    • Integrates early learning from environmental exposure.

Infant Speech Segmentation

  • Segmentation Problem: Infants must identify individual words within a continuous stream of spoken language without explicit boundaries.

  • Strategies:

    • Statistical Learning:

      • Saffran et al. (1996) showed infants track syllable co-occurrence probabilities.

      • High transitional probabilities indicate syllables belong to the same word; low probabilities indicate word boundaries.

    • Phonotactic Constraints:

      • Infants become sensitive to permissible sound sequences in their language.

      • Example: English-speaking infants learn that "ng" is unlikely at the beginning of words.

    • Prosodic Bootstrapping:

      • Infants use rhythm, stress, and intonation to infer word boundaries.

      • English two-syllable words often have a trochaic stress pattern (strong-weak).

      • By 7.5 months, infants use this pattern to segment speech, preferentially extracting words beginning with a stressed syllable.

    • Familiar Words as Anchors:

      • Learned words serve as reference points to segment adjacent speech.

Infant Word-Meaning Association

  • Age: Begins around 10-12 months.

  • Fast Mapping:

    • Children form a connection between a new word and its referent after minimal exposure.

    • Carey and Bartlett (1978) demonstrated that children could remember a novel word's meaning after hearing it once in a clear context.

  • Innate Biases and Heuristics:

    • Mutual Exclusivity: Children assume each object has only one label, ruling out known terms when hearing a new word.

    • Syntactic Bootstrapping: Inferring word meaning based on grammatical structure.

    • Pragmatic Cues: Using gaze direction and speaker intention.

  • Vocabulary Development:

    • Non-linear trajectory with U-shaped learning patterns.

    • Initially using correct forms, then overgeneralizing rules (e.g., “goed” instead of “went”), and eventually returning to correct usage after learning exceptions.

  • Cognitive Mechanisms: Combining domain-specific tools (attention to language) and domain-general cognitive mechanisms (categorisation and memory).

Critical Period Hypothesis (CPH) for Language Acquisition

  • Definition: There is a biologically constrained window (before puberty) during which language acquisition is most efficient.

  • Impact: If a child is not exposed to a language during this time, full native-like proficiency may be impossible, especially in syntax and phonology.

  • Evidence:

    • Case Studies: Genie, who was isolated until age 13, failed to acquire normal grammatical structures.

    • Late Sign Language Learners: Perform worse on syntactic tasks compared to those exposed from birth.

    • Neuroscientific Studies: Early language exposure leads to typical lateralisation and activation patterns in language-related brain areas; late learners often show more diffuse or right-hemisphere activation.

    • Cochlear Implant Studies: Children who receive implants earlier acquire language more effectively.

  • Conclusion: Brain plasticity for language is time-sensitive, and language input during the critical period is crucial for native-like mastery.

Critical Period for Second Language (L2) Acquisition

  • Complexity: More complex than L1 acquisition.

  • Evidence:

    • Earlier exposure may lead to more native-like proficiency, especially in pronunciation and grammar.

    • Johnson and Newport (1989) found a decline in grammatical performance among immigrants learning English after puberty.

  • Counter-Evidence: Birdsong and Molis (2001) found some adults achieved high levels of grammatical accuracy, suggesting compensation through motivation, input quality, and frequency of use.

  • Sensitive Period Model: While younger learners have advantages, successful L2 acquisition in adulthood is possible, particularly in immersive environments.

  • Bilingualism: Experience with multiple languages enhances cognitive flexibility and metalinguistic awareness.

  • Conclusion: Age interacts with social, psychological, and experiential variables in shaping L2 outcomes.

Shared Processes in Language Production Models

  • Models: Levelt, Garrett, Ellis & Young.

  • Fundamental Stages:

    • Conceptualisation: Forming a communicative intent.

    • Lexical Selection: Choosing the appropriate lemma.

    • Phonological Encoding: Retrieving the lexeme or phonological form.

    • Articulation: Executing the speech motor plan.

  • Tip-of-the-Tongue (TOT) States: Speakers retrieve the concept and grammatical properties of a word but fail to access its phonological form.

  • Neuropsychological and Experimental Evidence: Different types of aphasia illustrate how damage to specific stages results in predictable impairments.

    • Broca’s aphasia affects syntactic encoding.

    • Anomia reflects lexical retrieval problems.

  • Functional Imaging Studies: Distinct activation patterns correspond to each processing stage.

Aphasic and Split-Brain Patients: Naming Insights

  • Broca’s Aphasia: Slow, effortful speech with preserved comprehension; indicating damage to regions for grammatical processing and articulation.

  • Wernicke’s Aphasia: Fluent but semantically incoherent speech; showing deficit in lexical-semantic access.

  • Anomic Aphasia: Struggle to retrieve specific words despite understanding their meanings; which reinforce independence of semantic and phonological processes.

  • Split-Brain Studies: Left hemisphere is dominant for language production.

    • Sperry and Gazzaniga’s studies show patients may name objects presented to the right visual field (processed by the LH) but not the left visual field (processed by the RH).

  • Conclusion: Naming involves coordinated processes between visual, lexical, and articulatory systems, primarily lateralised to the left hemisphere.

Dual-Route Model of Reading

  • Mechanisms:

    • Lexical Route: Direct recognition of whole words from the mental lexicon; facilitates reading of irregular words (e.g., “choir,” “colonel”).

    • Non-Lexical Route: Grapheme-to-phoneme conversion; enables decoding of regular and novel words (e.g., “mint,” “flirp”).

  • Neuropsychological Evidence:

    • Surface Dyslexia: Damage to the lexical route results in difficulty reading irregular words, leading to regularisation errors (e.g., reading “pint” to rhyme with “mint”).

    • Phonological Dyslexia: Damage to the non-lexical route impairs ability to read non-words; familiar words remain accessible via the lexical route.

  • Functional Imaging Studies: Distinct neural pathways; temporal lobe regions support lexical access, and inferior parietal areas are involved in phonological conversion.

Visual Word Form Area (VWFA) in Reading

  • Location: Left fusiform gyrus.

  • Role: Facilitates rapid recognition of orthographic patterns.

  • Literate Individuals: Activates robustly during reading tasks, interfacing with phonological and semantic networks.

  • Illiterate Individuals: Shows reduced or absent activation during reading tasks.

  • Plasticity: Braille readers show VWFA responsiveness to tactile reading, indicating its tuning to symbolic input relevant to language.

  • Literacy Training: illiterate adults Leads to measurable increases in VWFA activity.

  • Conclusion: Reading recruits and repurposes existing brain regions through structured exposure and practice, highlighting that it is not an innate capacity.