Understand how delay affects the value/efficacy of a reinforcer:
In learning
In stable choice
Analyze how delay's impact on reinforcer value depends on environmental and organism characteristics.
Grasp how delay creates barriers (and can be useful) for habit change.
Delay reduces the effectiveness of reinforcers.
Key Points:
Delay discounts the value of reinforcers.
Learning is slower with delayed reinforcement (Wilkenfield et al., 1992).
Individuals favor immediate over delayed reinforcers in stable choice (Davison & Baum, 2007).
Wilkenfield et al. (1992): Studies with rats showed that a steeper slope in cumulative responses indicates faster acquisition of behavior.
Five groups of rats trained with varying delays in reinforcement.
More cumulative responses suggest quicker learning with immediate reinforcement.
Experiment by Davison and Baum (2007):
Executed concurrent schedules with reinforcer delays for one response and immediate for another.
Showed that delay shifts choice, impacting behavior significantly.
Utilization of delayed reinforcement can modify behavior:
Seatbelt use decreased with an 8-16 second delay in seatbelt reminders among drivers.
Vending machines using a 25-second delay on unhealthy options prompted healthier choices (Appelhans et al., 2018).
Green et al. (1981) study:
Preference shifted towards larger-later alternatives when the reinforcement was delayed, demonstrating how time proximity influences preferences between immediate and delayed rewards.
Allowing individuals to learn tolerance to delay.
Understanding reinforcement contingencies in current behaviors.
Teaching alternative behaviors and self-control techniques.
A choice between a smaller amount now and a larger amount later reflects the subjective value of delayed reinforcers.
The indifference point is where preference between smaller and larger rewards shifts.
Collecting subjective values across various delays creates a discounting function illustrating the impact of delay on reinforcer value.
Individuals discount at varying rates, influencing their impulsivity:
More steep discounting is associated with addictions (e.g., substance dependence, gambling).
Less discounting correlates with successful quitting and healthy behaviors (e.g., Bickel et al., 2018).
Situational contexts (e.g., gambling vs non-gambling) alter impulsivity.
Study by Dixon et al. (2006): Participants exhibit more impulsivity in gambling settings.
Kidd et al. (2013) illustrated the impact of environmental reliability on delay behavior in children:
Reliability in timing increased the likelihood of longer waiting for a reward.
Green et al. (1996): Age and income influence discounting behavior:
Older adults and those in lower income brackets discount values more steeply.
Natural environments promote healthier choices. Berry et al. (2014) found environments with nature yielded less inclination towards sugary drinks.
Episodic foresight reduces impulsivity and enhances future-oriented decision-making.
Delay diminishes the efficacy of reinforcers.
Delay produces a hyperbolic reduction in subjective value.
Individual and environmental contexts significantly influence the degree of discounting.
Discuss implications of immediate feedback (good/bad) on behavior change in restaurant staff.
Delay impacts reinforcer effectiveness across different species, supported by evidence.
Discuss hyperbolic functions in describing delay discounting and measurement of individual discounting rates.
Examine experiments illustrating environmental effects on delay discounting.
Concurrent Chain Schedule: Schedule involving two or more choices at the same time.
Indifference: State of having no preference between options.
Subjective Value: Perceived value of a delayed outcome.
Delay Discounting: Reduction in perceived value of a reward as its delay increases.
Hyperbolic Discounting: Function modeling the decrease in subjective value as the delay before obtaining a reward increases.