AD

Notes: Health, Stress, and Social Psychology – Key Studies and Concepts

  • Health, psychology, and health outcomes

    • Health can be affected by physiological factors and by the explanations we make for events, interactions, and outcomes in our lives.
    • Self-serving explanations and optimistic patterns are linked to better health outcomes in some studies.
    • Evidence across multiple studies suggests a link between cognitive/interpretive styles and longevity or recovery.
  • Key longitudinal and cross-sectional findings linking psychology to health

    • Baseball study (press releases, 94 famous players)
    • Pattern: players who made more pessimistic evaluations for losses tended to die earlier on average.
    • Implication: pessimistic explanations may reflect a broader pattern of appraisal that harms health over time.
    • Heart surgery recovery and optimism
    • More optimistic individuals tended to recover more quickly after heart surgery than more pessimistic individuals.
    • Nun longitudinal study (ages around 22 to 32 follow-up)
    • Positive affect (less grumpiness/negativity) at age 22 predicted longer life by about six years on average.
    • Positive individuals were about half as likely to suffer a heart attack ten years later compared with more negative peers.
    • 1920 Scottish cohort study (children born in 1920; IQ measured in childhood)
    • IQ and longevity: higher early-life IQ predicted longer life.
    • Finding: a 15-point increase in IQ was associated with a roughly 20% increase in longevity.
    • Possible explanations: higher IQ may aid better health information processing and healthier choices; causality remains complex.
    • Overall takeaway: appraisal style, optimism, and cognitive factors (including IQ) relate to health and longevity.
  • Stress and Lazarus’ cognitive appraisal model (1960s onward)

    • Core idea: stress arises when perceived resources are outweighed by demands.
    • Appraisals influence stress response: how we evaluate events determines how stressful they feel.
    • Primary appraisals (evaluate the source):
    • Neutral: everyday events perceived as neutral.
    • Positive: perceivable benefits or positive aspects.
    • Negative (harm/loss): potential loss from a situation (e.g., losing marks on a quiz).
    • Threat: demands are high; potential for harm to self or status; risk to self or reputation.
    • Challenge: demands are high but potentially solvable; opportunity to grow.
    • Secondary appraisals (resources to cope):
    • Do I have the needed resources to meet the demand? Personal resources (skills, knowledge) and social resources (supportive friends/family).
    • If both primary and secondary appraisals indicate insufficient resources to meet demands, stress is likely to be higher.
    • Key point: stress responses depend on both the situation and the person’s appraisals and resources, which can vary over time.
  • Coping with stress: two broad styles

    • Problem-focused coping: address the source of stress directly when possible (e.g., study for an exam, complete assignments).
    • Benefits: reduces perceived stress by actively reducing the source.
    • Example: procrastination as a coping pattern can be dysfunctional; as assignments pile up, procrastinators can experience increased stress and illness.
    • Emotion-focused coping: regulate emotional responses when the stressor cannot be addressed directly (e.g., illness).
    • Often more effective when the stressor is uncontrollable.
    • Resilience as a form of coping: individuals with higher resilience handle extreme stressors (e.g., 9/11) better than less resilient individuals.
    • Mixed evidence about which coping style is most effective; context (controllability of stressor) matters.
    • Experimental evidence (hostage-like scenario; 57 airline employees):
    • Participants assigned to problem-focused vs. emotion-focused coping strategies.
    • In highly controllable or non-controllable stress contexts, emotion-focused coping tended to be more effective when the source was largely uncontrollable.
  • Real-world demonstrations of coping and health-related outcomes

    • Two main forms of coping training in high-stress scenarios:
    • Problem-focused coping training (e.g., teamwork, problem solving).
    • Emotion-focused coping training (e.g., relaxation, controlled breathing).
    • In simulated abduction experiments (four-day isolation with terror-like scenarios), emotion-focused coping was more effective than problem-focused coping because the situation offered little direct control.
  • Social psychology of mental health and environment

    • Regional and migrational stress and mental health (UK):
    • Incidence of schizophrenia varies by town size and region; double in some South London areas compared to Nottingham and Bristol.
    • Afro-Caribbean migrants in the UK had a ninefold higher likelihood of schizophrenia compared to other parts of Britain.
    • Rates in the West Indies and Britain’s migrant populations pointed away from simple country-of-origin explanations; migrant stress and environmental factors appeared central.
    • Higher schizophrenia rates correlated with lower public health spending and resources, indicating environmental stressors contribute to mental health outcomes.
  • Group dynamics: conformity, obedience, and group processes

    • Conformity: tendency to alter perceptions, opinions, or behavior to fit group norms.
    • Informational influence: accepting information from others as evidence about reality when a task is difficult or ambiguous (desire to be correct).
    • Normative influence: conforming to fit in with the group to avoid social disapproval or rejection, even when you may know the correct answer.
    • Norms: shared beliefs within a group; promote cohesion but can drive negative outcomes if they suppress dissent.
  • Classic demonstrations of conformity and related concepts

    • Sherif (autokinesis) and informational influence:
    • In a dark room, a fixed light appears to move due to eye movements (autokinesis).
    • Participants estimated movement alone or in groups; over trials, ratings converged to a common mean, illustrating informational influence.
    • Asch paradigm (normative influence):
    • Simple perceptual line-length task where the correct answer is obvious.
    • In group settings, 75% of participants conformed at least once; over one-third of all responses were conforming.
    • Manipulations:
      • Anonymous writing condition (responses written rather than spoken) reduced conformity to about a third of original levels.
      • The presence of allies who gave the correct answer dramatically reduced conformity (one ally cut conformity to ~25% of original).
      • Group size effects: with two confederates, conformity rose (e.g., around 13.6%); with three confederates, it rose further; after about eight participants, conformity plateaued.
      • Allied condition: one confederate giving the correct answer reduced conformity substantially, even though others gave incorrect answers.
    • Distinction within conformity: observationally different pathways to conformity include normative conformity (driven by desire to fit in) and informational conformity (driven by a desire to be correct).
  • Obedience and the power of authority

    • Compliance vs. obedience: compliance is responding to a direct request; obedience is complying with a direct order from an authority figure.
    • Milgram's obedience studies (classic and influential):
    • Setup: teacher (participant) instructed to administer increasingly painful electric shocks to a learner for wrong answers; shocks administered through a separate room with the learner in another room.
    • Voltage sequence: starting at ~${45}$ volts, increasing per wrong answer up to ${450}$ volts; the learner’s protests increased in intensity.
    • Key finding (initial study): about ${65 ext{%}}$ of participants administered the maximum shock (${450}$ V) despite clear distress signals from the learner.
    • Other outcomes: ~${5 ext{%}}$ stopped at around ${120}$ volts; some stopped later as the learner complained; the experimenter insisted on continuing, using a firm authority stance.
    • Temporal effects: the experiment showed the power of situational factors in forcing people to obey an authority even when it conflicts with personal conscience.
    • Ethical concerns: the study had profound psychological impact on participants; led to many ethical restrictions in subsequent research.
    • Replications and contemporary interpretations
    • Burger (2009) replication up to ${150}$ volts: no significant difference from Milgram’s initial patterns; ~${82.5 ext{%}}$ continued beyond ${150}$ V in the replication vs. ~${75 ext{%}}$ in some replications.
    • Modern replications in BBC contexts and other settings have reproduced the general obedience pattern, though with modified voltages and safeguards.
    • Some participants believed they had administered shocks even when not happening; the belief itself had ethical implications.
    • Implications for obedience research: situational factors (authority presence, proximity, ceremonial cues like lab coats) can strongly influence obedience; distance from the learner (physical or psychological) reduces perceived harm and increases compliance.
  • Group processes and organizational behavior

    • Groupthink (Janis): pathological group dynamics where the desire for consensus overrides realistic appraisal of alternatives.
    • Bay of Pigs example: Kennedy’s era, illusion of invulnerability; strong emphasis on consensus; suppression of dissent; stereotypes about strength of allies and opponents; need to maintain group cohesion; voting to evaluate counterarguments reinforced the group’s prior view.
    • Consequences: poor planning, failure to consider alternatives or what would happen if plans failed.
    • Other examples of groupthink pressures
    • Challenger accident: o-ring failure; post-accident analyses suggested engineers warned about risk, but information failed to reach top decision-makers in a timely way; a culture of urgency and consensus suppressed critical dissent.
    • Stanford Prison Experiment (Zimbardo)
    • Setup: 24 male undergraduates randomly assigned to guard or prisoner roles in a mock prison in the basement of Stanford’s psychology building; planned for two weeks.
    • Reality: within a week, abusive dynamics between guards and prisoners emerged; study terminated early due to escalation and ethical concerns; Zimbardo’s immersion in the role as “prison governor” blinded him to the emerging harm.
    • Takeaway: powerful situational factors can shape behavior, authority dynamics, and group roles; the line between experimenter and role-occupant blur in high-arousal environments.
    • Practical caution: these studies illustrate how environments and norms can shape behavior, sometimes leading to extreme outcomes; replication today is heavily restricted for ethical reasons.
  • Bystander effect and collective responsiveness to danger

    • Kitty Genovese case (1960s) and media debate
    • Reports described 38 witnesses who either did not intervene or call authorities over a period of more than half an hour, raising questions about public apathy.
    • Later scrutiny highlighted uncertainties about the events; nonetheless, the case became a touchstone for bystander research.
    • Latane and Darley’s bystander research
    • Core finding: with multiple bystanders present, individuals are less likely to help compared with when there is a single bystander.
    • Explanations include informational influence (people use others’ inaction as a cue for how to behave in ambiguous situations) and diffusion of responsibility.
    • Explanations and moderating factors
    • People with well-developed professional models (police, paramedics) show reduced bystander effects due to familiarity with emergency protocols.
    • In ambiguous or rare extreme situations, people often look to others for cues, leading to inaction unless someone takes initiative.
  • Summary of the interconnections among psychology, health, and group dynamics

    • Psychological appraisals, resilience, and cognitive factors relate to physical and mental health outcomes.
    • Stress coping styles (problem-focused vs. emotion-focused) interact with controllability and context to influence health outcomes.
    • Social context (conformity, obedience, groupthink, bystander effects) can powerfully shape behavior in everyday life and in crisis situations alike, with implications for health, safety, and social welfare.
  • Formulas and key statistics to remember

    • Longevity and IQ relationship: a 15-point increase in IQ is associated with a ≈ 20% increase in longevity. ext{IQ} o ext{Longevity}: ext{ΔIQ}=+15
      ightarrow rac{ ext{Longevity}{ ext{new}}- ext{Longevity}{ ext{old}}}{ ext{Longevity}_{ ext{old}}} imes 100 \approx ext{+20 ext%}
    • Nun study: positivity at age 22 predicted +6 years of life; those with positive appraisals were about half as likely to have a heart attack a decade later.
    • UK schizophrenia regional patterns: schizophrenia incidence is doubled in some parts of South London relative to Nottingham/Bristol; Afro-Caribbean migrants to the UK have ≈ ninefold higher risk than other Brits; higher risk associated with lower public health spending.
    • Milgram obedience (original study): about 65% of participants delivered the maximum shock (450 V); approximately 5% stopped at ~120 V; over 80% continued past the initial distress; the majority obeyed to the end despite learner distress.
    • Burger replication (2009): no significant difference from Milgram’s pattern beyond the 150 V threshold; about 82.5% exceeded 150 V in some variants vs about 75% in other replications; ethical safeguards reduced extreme exposure.
    • Asch conformity: 75% conformed at least once; more than one-third of all responses were conformist; anonymity and presence of a dissenter (ally) reduced conformity markedly (to about 27–25% of original levels depending on condition).
    • Group size effects in Asch: conformity increased with more confederates up to a point; two confederates produced ~13.6% conformity; three confederates higher; plateau around eight participants.
    • Bystander effect: multiple bystanders reduce likelihood of intervention; single bystander more likely to intervene than multiple bystanders.
  • Practical implications and ethical considerations highlighted in the lecture

    • The powerful impact of situational factors on behavior underscores the caution needed when interpreting actions as purely individual choices.
    • Ethical considerations in social psychology experiments have evolved; Milgram’s and Zimbardo’s studies prompted stricter protections for participants and more rigorous ethical review processes.
    • Real-world relevance: understanding conformity, obedience, groupthink, and bystander effects helps explain historical events (Bay of Pigs, Challenger) and informs modern policy, organizational management, and crisis response.
  • Connections to prior and future topics

    • Links between appraisal theory, stress, and health tie into broader health psychology and behavioral medicine.
    • Conformity, obedience, and group processes provide a bridge to organizational behavior, leadership, and crisis management.
    • The bystander effect connects to public health interventions and crowd safety planning.
  • Hypothetical scenarios and takeaways for exams

    • If faced with an exam stressor, consider primary appraisal (is this threat or challenge?) and secondary appraisal (do I have the resources to cope?). Choose coping strategies accordingly (problem-focused if you can act; emotion-focused if the stressor is uncontrollable).
    • In a group decision scenario, evaluate potential groupthink risks: is there unnecessary cohesion? Are dissenting opinions being heard? Is there role of leadership suppressing criticism?
    • In a crisis, recognize the potential for the bystander effect: ensure clear cues for action, designate bystander roles, and provide simple commands to reduce diffusion of responsibility.
  • Key terms to memorize

    • Appraisal: primary vs secondary; threat vs loss vs challenge; resources and social support.
    • Coping: problem-focused vs emotion-focused; resilience.
    • Conformity: informational vs normative influence; normative influence often involves fear of social disapproval.
    • Obedience vs compliance: authority-driven behavior; Milgram’s paradigm and its ethical implications.
    • Groupthink: Janis; Bay of Pigs; Challenger.
    • Bystander effect: Latane & Darley; Kitty Genovese.
    • Autokinesis: Sherif; information-driven convergence.
    • IQ and health: longevity link; possible mechanisms (health information processing, behavior).
  • Quick references to remember from the transcript

    • n = 94 baseball players; pessimistic loss explanations linked to earlier mortality.
    • Nun study: positivity at age 22 → +6 years longevity; odds of heart attack at 32 halved for positive nuns.
    • Scottish cohort: IQ at age ~11 predicts longevity; 15-point IQ increase → ≈20% longer life.
    • UK schizophrenia: Afro-Caribbean migrants ≈ 9× risk; South London regions ≈ 2× risk vs other UK regions; higher public health spending associated with lower schizophrenia rates.
    • Milgram: 65% reached 450 V; 5% stopped around 120 V; 80% continued beyond distress; experimenter authority cue.
    • Burger replication (2009): similar obedience patterns with ethical safeguards.
    • Asch: 75% conformed at least once; anonymity reduced conformity; one ally reduced conformity to ~25% of baseline; larger groups increased conformity up to plateau.
    • Zimbardo: Stanford Prison Experiment; guards’ abuse led to early termination after 1 week.
    • Janis: Bay of Pigs; emphasis on consensus, invulnerability, suppression of dissent; critique as an example of groupthink.
    • Challenger: groupthink dynamics; failure to properly heed warnings about O-rings.
    • Bystander: Kitty Genovese; bystander effect via informational influence; professional responders less susceptible due to established models.