What Exactly is Behavior?
All animal behaviors are based on physiological systems and processes.
Behavior is the sum of an organism’s responses to external and internal stimuli.
In animals, behaviors are typically actions carried out by muscles under control of the nervous system.
Examples include an animal releasing a scent to mark its territory or using its throat muscles to produce a song.
Behavior is an essential part of acquiring nutrients and finding a partner for sexual reproduction.
It also contributes to homeostasis, a state of balance among all the body systems needed for the body to survive and function correctly.
In short, all of physiology contributes to behavior, and behavior influences all of physiology.
Many behaviors, especially involving recognition and communication, rely on specialized body structures or form.
Natural selection that shapes behaviors also influences the evolution of animal anatomy.
What are the Broader Functions of Animal Behaviors?
Niko Tinbergen, a Dutch scientist and a pioneer in the study of animal behavior, asked several questions on animal behavior:
What stimulus elicits the behavior, and what physiological mechanisms mediate the response?
How does the animal’s experience during growth and development influence the response?
How does the behavior make the animal more likely to survive and reproduce?
What is the behavior’s evolutionary history?
As part of his researched, he kept fish tanks of three-spined stickback fishes.
In this species, only males have red bellies.
Male sticklebacks attack other males who invade in their nesting territories.
Tinbergen noticed male sticklebacks behaved aggressively when a red truck passed by their view.
He hypothesized that the color red was the cause of the attack behavior.
They will not attack a fish lacking red coloration.
The territorial response of male sticklebacks is an example of a fixed action pattern.
These is a sequence of unlearned acts directly linked to a simple stimulus.
These patterns are essentially unchangeable and are usually carried to completion.
The trigger for the behavior is an external cue called a sign stimulus.
Environmental stimuli also provides cues that animals use to carry out behaviors.
For example, many birds, fishes, and other animals use environmental cues to guide migration - a regular, long-distance change in location.
Some migrating animals track their position relative to the sun, even though the sun’s position relative to Earth changes throughout the day.
Animals can adjust for these changes by a means of a circadian clock, an internal mechanism that maintains a 24-hour activity rhythm or cycle.
The clock is responsible for a circadian rhythm, a daily cycle of rest and activity.
Some behaviors like migration and reproduction reflect biological rhythms with a longer cycle, or period, than the circadian rhythm.
Behavioral rhythms linked to the yearly cycle of seasons are called circannual rhythms.
How do Animals Communicate?
A stimulus transmitted from one organism to another is called a signal.
The transmission and reception of signals between animals constitute communication, which often has a role of proximate causation of behavior.
There are 4 common modes of animal communication:
Visual, chemical, tactile, and auditory.
For example, a fruit fly courtship is an example of a stimulus-response chain, in which the response to each stimulus is itself the stimulus for the next behavior.
Male fly detects female and orients his body towards him, and then uses his senses to detect chemicals she releases in the air.
Then he approaches and touches her foreleg.
The male then extends and vibrates one wing, producing a courtship song.
This shows that both the male and female are of the same species.
If successful, the female will allow the male to attempt copulation.
A rule of thumb is that the form of communication that evolves is closely related to an animal’s lifestyle and environment.
Like visual displays are not so effective with nocturnal animals.
So they rely on auditory signals, which works well in both the dark and light.
It was discovered in the early 1900s by Austrian researcher Karl von Frisch that European honeybees have a unique way of communicating.
He and he students observed that the bees’ movements make up a “dance language” that returning foragers use to inform other bees about the distance and direction of travel to food sources.
Animals that communicate through odors or tastes emit chemical substances called pheromones.
Pheromones are often related to reproductive behavior.
An example from earlier is the fruit fly courtship ritual in which the female releases pheromones.
Pheromones are not just short-distance signaling, as it can also be used for long-distance as well.
Male silkworm moths can detect female moths’ pheromones from several kilometers away.
Pheromones can also serve as alarm signals.
How do Animals Learn their Behaviors?
For some behaviors, nearly all individuals in a population behave alike.
Such as a fixed action pattern, a courtship stimulus-response chain, or pheromone signaling.
Behavior that is developmentally fixed in this way is known as innate behavior.
One powerful way that an animal’s environment can influence its behavior is through learning.
Learning is the modification of behavior as a result of specific experiences.
The capacity for learning depends on nervous system organization established during development.
It involves the formation of memories by specific changes in neuronal connectivity.
It also follows instructions encoded in the genome.
The ability of offspring to recognize and be recognized by a parent is essential for survival.
In the young, learning comes rom imprinting, the establishment of a long-lasting behavioral response to a particular individual or object.
Imprinting can take place only during a specific time period in development.
This is called the sensitive period.
During the sensitive period, the young imprint on their parents and learn basic behaviors.
The parents also learn to recognize its offspring.
If bonding does not occur, the parent will not care for the offspring, and this leads to the offspring’s death and a decrease in reproductive success of the parents.
How else do Animals Learn?
Every natural environment has spatial variation.
These include locations of nests, hazards, food, and prospective mates.
An organism’s fitness may be enhanced by the capacity for spatial learning, the establishment of a memory that reflects the environment’s spatial structure.
For example, Tinbergen wanted to test how animals used spatial awareness by experimenting on a female digger wasp.
The wasp covers the entrance of her underground nest and memorizes it by learning visual landmarks that marks her location, in which pinecones are used.
Tinbergen then displaced the pinecones away from the nest, and when the wasp returned, she flew to the center of the pinecone circle.
In some animals, spatial learning involves formulating a cognitive map.
A cognitive map is a representation in an animal’s nervous system of the spatial relationships between objects in its surroundings.
By experimentally varying the distance between landmarks in the birds’ environments, researchers found that birds used the halfway point between landmarks rather than a fixed distance to find their hidden food stores.
Learning also involves making associations between experiences.
Associative learning is the acquired ability to associate one environmental feature (like color) with another (like danger).
Associative learning doesn’t just have to be colors.
Research shows that some pigeons can associate danger with sound but not with a color.
Conversely, a rat also can learn to avoid illness-inducing foods on the basis on smells, not with sights or sounds.
The associations an animal can readily form usually reflects relationships that often happens in nature.
And associations that can’t be formed are unlikely to be of selective advantage in a native environment.
The most complex forms of learning involve cognition - the process of knowing that involves awareness, reasoning, recollection, and judgment.
Problem solving is the cognitive activity of devising a method to proceed from one condition to another in the face of real or apparent obstacles.
Many animals learn to solve problems by observing the behavior of other individuals.
This type of learning through observing is called social learning.
Social learning forms the roots of culture, which can be defined as a system of information transfer through social learning or teaching that influences the behavior of individuals in a population.
Cultural transfer of information can alter behavior phenotypes and thereby influence the fitness of individuals.
What is Mating Behavior and Mate Choice?
Mating behavior and mate choice play a big role in determining reproductive success.
These include seeking or attracting mates, choosing among potential mates, competing for mates, and caring for offspring.
In the context of reproduction, biologists describe differences in mating systems among species, the length and number of relationships between males and females.
Some animals engage in monogamy (one male mating with one female) and others engage in polygamy (individual of one sex mating with several others).
Sexual dimorphism, the extent to which males and females differ in appearance, also varies with the type of mating system.
The needs of the young are an important factor constraining the evolution of mating systems.
Another factor influencing mating behavior and parental care is certainty of paternity.
The certainty of paternity is low in most species with internal fertilization because the acts of mating and birth are separated over time.
Certainty of paternity is high when egg laying and mating occur together, as in external fertilization.
Parental behavior correlated with certainty of paternity exists because it has been reinforced over generations by natural selection.
Mate preferences of females may play a central role in the evolution of male behavior and anatomy through intersexual selection.
For example, studies show that during courtship, female stalk-eyed flies are more likely to mate with males that have long eyestalks.
Things like this might correlate with health and vitality, which will help ensure the production of more offspring that survive to reproduce.
The example of stalk-eyed flies can show how female choice can select for one best type of male in a given situation.
This typically results in low variation among males.
Male competition for mates can reinforce the tendency for reduced variation among males.
This competition represents an agonistic behavior, a contest that determines which competitor gains access to a mate or other resource.