Evolutionary Perspectives on Male Attraction to Women

Evolutionary framework and mating goals

  • Men have an almost unlimited supply of sperm and a very long reproductive window from puberty into old age, making reproduction potentially continuous over much of life.

  • From an evolutionary perspective, the basic goal is to pass on genes to future generations; a simple, large-scale strategy for men is to have many offspring.

  • Therefore, what would be important for men when selecting a mate? Fertility: the ability to mate with someone who will produce offspring.

  • Implication: evolutionary theory predicts men should be attracted to cues of youth and fertility in women.

Facial features and attractiveness: estrogen, femininity, and signaling fertility

  • Estrogen and femininity cues in female faces are linked to perceived attractiveness in men.

  • Correlations reported in the literature:

    • Correlation between men's ratings of facial masculinity in men and women's ratings of male attractiveness: r \,\approx\, 0.35

    • Correlation between men's ratings of facial femininity in women and their attractiveness: r \,\approx\, 0.65

    • Interpretation: women rate the attractiveness of men partly based on masculine features, but men rate women more strongly on feminine facial cues.

  • Why might feminine facial features be attractive? Linked to estrogen levels.

    • Estrogen is dominant in women and peaks during fertility periods (20s–30s).

    • Estrogen contributes to features like shiny hair, plump rosy skin, and even influences body features discussed later.

    • Across the menstrual cycle, estrogen peaks at ovulation, with subtle facial changes (slightly plumper, rosier skin; lips may appear fuller).

    • These estrogenic changes may signal possible ovulation to men.

  • Cross-cultural consistency:

    • Standards of beauty (youth and fertility cues) are relatively consistent across cultures.

    • There is broad agreement on attractive female faces across many countries, supporting the idea of universal fertility cues.

Long-term vs short-term mating preferences and health signals

  • Across cultures, both men and women value physical attractiveness, but:

    • Men tend to place more emphasis on physical attractiveness when seeking a long-term mate than women do, though both value it.

    • Attractiveness correlates with health, which is important for both partners: a healthy woman can reproduce; a healthy man can rear offspring.

  • OkCupid data (book by Christian Rudder) illustrate relative ratings:

    • Women rate far fewer men as attractive (a peak at the low end of attractiveness), while men rate many more women as attractive.

    • This implies a small percentage of men receive most dating interaction (roughly 20% of men get most hits), while many women compete for those few highly ranked men.

    • Potential consequence: even highly desirable men may shift toward short-term mating strategies if social dynamics permit.

  • Takeaway: attractiveness is important in both sexes, but distributional dynamics create asymmetries that affect mating strategies and competition.

Body features: waist-to-hip ratio, lumbar curvature, and biomechanics

  • Waist-to-hip ratio (WHR): classic cue studied by Doctor Devendra Singh.

    • Manipulation: changing WHR in a photo from 0.70 (left) to 0.80 and 0.90 (right).

    • Finding: men rate WHR = 0.70 as most attractive across cultures.

    • Why 0.70? This ratio signals youth and reproductive potential; higher WHR (0.80–0.90) signals lower pregnancy probability and poorer health.

    • Note: WHR attractiveness is distinct from body size or weight; lean vs voluptuous bodies can have WHR around 0.70.

  • Cross-cultural depiction: Singh showed that art from various eras and cultures (Greek, Roman, African, Indian) routinely depicts beauty with WHR ≈ 0.70.

  • Lumbar curvature:

    • Research by Dr. David Lewis on optimal lumbar curvature for pregnancy: about 45 degrees (

    • Women with ~45° lumbar curvature are better at sustaining pregnancy; men tend to rate women with ~45° curvature as more attractive.

    • Practical observation: high heels exaggerate lumbar curvature and may have historically been perceived as attractive; no direct causal study linking heels to the curvature threshold discussed.

Eyes, gaze, and ocular cues

  • Eye size and youth signaling:

    • Infants have relatively large eyes; as people age, the proportion of eye size to overall face decreases.

    • Large eyes and wide-set eyes are judged highly attractive by men as signals of youth.

  • Pupil dilation as a cue of interest:

    • Pupils dilate when attracted; a classic demonstration shows larger pupils on a photo make it more attractive to viewers.

    • Ecological validity example: two identical textbook covers, one with enlarged pupils in the photo, led men to choose the book with the larger-pupil image.

  • Eye makeup: emphasis on eyes to appear larger is an ancient, cross-cultural practice linked to perceived beauty.

  • Sclera and limbal ring:

    • Whiter sclera (the white of the eye) is rated as more attractive because yellowness or redness can indicate illness.

    • Limbal ring: a dark ring around the iris; aging reduces the limbal ring, so a larger limbal ring is associated with youth and fertility.

    • The combination of large, white sclera with a prominent limbal ring is viewed as highly attractive.

Non-heterosexual considerations: orientation and cues

  • Gay men and attraction:

    • Gay men prioritize physical attractiveness similarly to heterosexual men when choosing partners (though the partner is male).

    • Body: lean, toned, athletic builds are preferred in gay men, with caveats for individual differences.

    • Faces: generally prefer masculine facial traits (prominent brow, wide jaw, large chin) akin to heterosexual men.

    • One study suggested more masculine gay men may prefer more feminine male faces, but replication is needed; conclusions are tentative.

    • Overall stance: the features judged attractive in men appear largely similar regardless of sexual orientation of the evaluator, but data remain limited.

  • Voice patterns in gays:

    • Some gay men display higher-pitched voice modulation and less breathy voices, not necessarily tied to testosterone levels; could be an in-group signaling pattern.

  • Attractiveness preferences in gay men for other men:

    • A study in China (338 gay men) found a preference for lower-pitched male voices (masculine-sounding voices) on average, though substantial individual variation exists.

Voice, scent, and ovulation cues: potential indicators of fertility

  • Ovulation and voice pitch:

    • Hypothesis: during ovulation, women may present cues indicating fertility; one aspect is a higher-pitched voice, rated as more attractive by heterosexual men.

    • Research by Marty Hazleton (Buss lab affiliate): women’s voices are higher pitched around ovulation and rated more attractive.

  • Hormonal changes and physical signals during the ovulatory phase:

    • Hormone graph: estrogen peaks around ovulation; testosterone also peaks near this time.

    • Mid-cycle estrogen increase leads to visible changes: skin glow, facial fullness, and slight lip fullness; waist-to-hip ratio may shift toward the fertility cue (0.70).

    • Breasts may become more symmetrical mid-cycle due to estrogen.

  • Odor cues and ovulation:

    • Singh and Bronstad: women wore t-shirts for three nights during ovulation and during the luteal phase; men sniffed the shirts and rated attractiveness higher for ovulatory odors.

    • Women on oral contraceptives did not show the same effect, suggesting estrogen cycling drives the cue.

  • Copulins and male attraction:

    • Copulins are pheromone-like compounds present in vaginal secretions that fluctuate with the cycle.

    • Experimental demonstration by Grammer and Oberze1rck: exposure to artificial copulins reduced men’s ability to discriminate attractiveness among women and caused a rise in testosterone.

    • The practical translation into real-world attraction is debated; replication and replication quality are limited.

    • Narrative aside: sensationalized portrayals in media (e.g., “butter gone off”) accompany these findings; scientific replication remains essential.

  • Scent, cognition, and ovulation:

    • Exposures to ovulatory cues may elevate male testosterone acutely, potentially altering judgments of attractiveness.

  • Ovulation and social/sexual behavior:

    • Several findings suggest that ovulation cues (olfactory, facial, and behavioral) may subtly increase women’s perceived attractiveness and social receptivity during peak fertility.

    • Yet some questions remain: are these cues reliably detectable by men, or do women become more receptive during ovulation leading to increased mating success?

  • Copulins and behavior: caution on interpretation

    • Copulins may influence male perception of attractiveness, but results are not universally replicated; the effects are small and context-dependent.

Voices and scent: effects across orientation and context

  • Heterosexual women and ovulation cues:

    • Women in the ovulatory phase may have higher sex drive; estrogen and testosterone link to arousal and drive (though findings are mixed and nuanced).

  • Gay men and vocal cues:

    • Some data suggest gay men may prefer lower-pitched male voices; however, there is substantial individual variability.

Context effects in attractiveness and mating choices

  • Availability and “closing time” effect in bars:

    • Pennebaker conducted field work in bars to test whether attractiveness ratings change over the course of the evening.

    • Finding: ratings of attractiveness increase as the night progresses, with a stronger effect for men than for women.

    • The phenomenon is called the “closing time effect” and is separate from the “beer goggles” theory.

    • Replication by Brian Gladue found evidence for beer goggles, but the closing time effect persisted beyond alcohol intoxication.

  • Cognitive dissonance explanation for closing time effect:

    • A dissonance-based account: men who had initially judged someone unattractive at closing time might re-evaluate to minimize the discomfort of going home alone.

    • Change in opinion (e.g., downgrading initial standards) reduces cognitive dissonance between desire and outcome.

  • Peralu’s “body vs. face” paradigm and short-term vs long-term mating:

    • Short-term mating: men preferred to see the body to glean cues of fertility and physical fitness.

    • Long-term mating: men preferred to see the face to judge personality, warmth, status, and potential for a lasting partnership.

    • Women showed the opposite pattern: in both contexts, 70% chose to view the face for attractiveness (long-term reasoning).

    • Interpretation: facial cues may convey personality and long-term compatibility beyond fertility cues.

  • Age preferences and mate value across cultures:

    • General pattern: women prefer men older than themselves; men prefer younger women.

    • Exceptions in polygynous societies (e.g., Zambia): men are older when they acquire multiple wives, hence greater age differences.

    • Teenage boys show a specific exception: they often prefer women who are slightly older (e.g., 18–20) due to peak fertility considerations at those ages.

  • OkCupid data on age preferences:

    • Men are attracted to partners who are roughly the same age as themselves (
      age of man = age of partner).

    • Women tend to prefer partners about five years older than themselves; there are cultural nuances and context effects.

  • Cultural and societal factors that shape attractiveness standards:

    • High parasite prevalence cultures tend to place greater emphasis on physical attractiveness because health signals are crucial in mate choice.

    • In contexts of food scarcity, heavier body weight can be preferred as a signal of resource availability and fertility potential.

    • Western emphasis on thinness may reflect resource availability rather than an ultimate fitness signal.

  • Practical implications and cautions:

    • Evolutionary theories offer interpretations for patterns in mate choice, but cultural, economic, and individual variation is substantial.

    • Replication and cross-cultural research are essential to validate specific links (e.g., ovulation cues, copulin effects).

    • Ethical and philosophical considerations apply when translating findings into real-world dating or social contexts.

Summary of major themes and implications

  • There is substantial cross-cultural support for the idea that cues to youth and fertility influence male attraction to females, especially in the domain of facial and body features.

  • Facial attractiveness correlates with femininity cues in women, and femininity is linked with estrogen levels and reproductive potential.

  • Physical cues such as WHR around 0.70 and a lumbar curvature near 45° are repeatedly associated with perceived attractiveness and pregnancy-related biomechanics.

  • The eyes (size, dilation, sclera whiteness, limbal ring) are robust cues of youth and health, and women utilize eye makeup to enhance perceived size.

  • Non-heterosexual orientations show similar patterns of attraction to physical aesthetics, with some unique voice preferences and in-group signaling tendencies; however, data are still limited and require replication.

  • Odor and scent cues (ovulatory shifts) may play a role in male attraction, with some robust findings (e.g., ovulatory odors increasing perceived attractiveness) but also significant replication concerns and dependence on hormonal status (oral contraception dampens effects).

  • Cognitive and contextual factors (availability, closing-time effects, and short-term vs. long-term mating goals) significantly modulate attractiveness judgments and partner preferences.

  • Cross-cultural variability exists, particularly in health signals, disease parasite prevalence, and resource availability, affecting the weight given to physical attractiveness and body signals.

  • Ethical and practical implications abound when applying these insights to real-world dating scenarios; interpretation should consider replication status and cultural context.

Bonus considerations and future directions

  • Many findings rely on correlations or single studies; replication across diverse populations is essential.

  • Future research could test causal pathways (e.g., do ovulatory cues cause men to act more on mating motives, or do women alter signals to increase mating opportunities?)

  • Interventions or education might address misinterpretations of attractiveness research in media and dating apps, especially given culture-specific norms.

  • Exploration of how hormonal contraception alters attractiveness cues and mate preferences remains a critical area for understanding fertility signaling.