No species can see in the dark, but some are capable of seeing when there is little light.
Light can be thought of as:
Particles of energy (photons)
Waves of electromagnetic radiation
Humans see light between 380–760 nanometers.
Wavelength: perception of color
Intensity: perception of brightness
Light enters the eye through the pupil, whose size changes in response to changes in illumination
Sensitivity: the ability to see when light is dim
Acuity: the ability to see details
Lens: focuses light on the retina
Ciliary muscles alter the shape of the lens as \n needed.
Accommodation: the process of adjusting the \n lens to bring images into focus
Convergence: eyes must turn slightly inward when objects are close
Binocular disparity: the difference between the images on the two retinas
Both are greater when objects are close—together, they provide the brain with a 3-D image and distance information.
The retina is, in a sense, inside-out.
Light passes through several cell layers before reaching its receptors.
Vertical pathway: receptors > bipolar cells > retinal ganglion cells
Lateral Communication
Horizontal cells
Amacrine cells
Blind spot: no receptors where information exits the eye
The visual system uses information from cells around the blind spot for “completion,” filling in the blind spot.
Fovea: high-acuity area at center of retina
Thinning of the ganglion cell layer reduces distortion due to cells between the pupil and the retina.
Duplexity theory of vision: cones and rod mediate different kinds of vision.
Cones: photopic (daytime) vision
High-acuity color information in good lighting
Rods: scotopic (nighttime) vision
High-sensitivity, allowing for low-acuity vision in dim \n light, but lacks detail and color information
There is more convergence in the rod system, \n increasing sensitivity while decreasing acuity.
Only cones are found at the fovea.
Lights of the same intensity but different wavelengths may not all look equally bright.
A spectral sensitivity curve shows the relationship between wavelength and brightness.
There are different spectral sensitivity curves for photopic (cone) vision and scotopic (rod) vision.
We continually scan the world with small and quick eye movements: saccades.
These bits of information are then integrated.
Stabilize retinal image; see nothing
The visual system responds to change.
Transduction: conversion of one form of energy to another
Visual transduction: conversion of light to neural signals by visual receptors
Pigments absorb light.
Absorption spectrum describes spectral sensitivity.Rhodopsin is the pigment found in rods.
A G-protein-linked receptor that responds to light rather than to neurotransmitters.
In the Dar
Na+ channels remain partially open (partial depolarization), releasing glutamate.
When Light Strike
Na+ channels close
Rods hyperpolarize, inhibiting glutamate release.
The retinal-geniculate-striate pathways include \n about 90 percent of axons of retinal ganglion cells.
The left hemiretina of each eye (right visual field) connects to the right lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN); the right hemiretina (left visual field) connects to the left LGN.
Most LGN neurons that project to primary visual cortex (V1, striate cortex) terminate in the lower part of cortical layer IV.
Information received at adjacent portions of the retina remains adjacent in the striate cortex (retinotopic).
More cortex is devoted to areas of high acuity—like the disproportionate representation of sensitive body parts in somatosensory cortex.
About 25 percent of primary visual cortex is dedicated to input from the fovea.
Magnocellular Layers (M Layers)
Big cell bodies; bottom two layers of LGN
Particularly responsive to movement
Input primarily from rods
Parvocellular Layers (P Layers)
Small cell bodies; top four layers of LGN
Color, detail, and still or slow objects
Input primarily from cones
The channels project to slightly different areas in lower layer IV in striate cortex; M neurons are just above the P neurons.
The channels project to different parts of visual cortex beyond V1.
Contrast Enhancement
Mach bands: nonexistent stripes the visual system creates for contrast enhancement
Makes edges easier to see
A consequence of lateral inhibition
The Area of the Visual Field within which It Is Possible for a Visual Stimulus to Influence the Firing of a Given Neuron
Hubel and Wiesel looked at receptive fields in the retinal ganglion, LGN, and lower layer IV of striate cortex of a cat.
Similarities seen at all three levels:
Receptive fields of foveal areas are smaller than those in the periphery.
Neurons’ receptive fields are circular in shape.
Neurons are monocular.
Many neurons at each level had receptive fields with excitatory and inhibitory area.
Many cells have receptive fields with a center \n -surround organization: excitatory and inhibitory regions separated by a circular boundary.
Some cells are on-center and some are off-center.
In lower layer IV of the striate cortex, neurons with circular receptive fields (as in retinal ganglion cells and LGN) are rare.
Most neurons in V1 are either:
Simple—receptive fields are rectangular with “on” and “off” regions—or
Complex—also rectangular, with larger receptive fields, and respond best to a particular stimulus anywhere in their receptive fields
SIMPLE
Rectangular
“On” and “off” regions, like cells in layer IV
Orientation and location sensitive
All are monocular.
COMPLEX
Rectangular
Larger receptive fields
Do not have static “on” and “off” regions
Not location sensitive
Motion sensitive
Many are binocular.
Cells with simpler receptive fields send information on to cells with more complex receptive fields.
Functional vertical columns exist such that all cells in a column have the same receptive field and ocular dominance.
Ocular dominance columns: as you move horizontally, the dominance of the columns changes.
Retinotopic organization is maintained.
Component Theory (Trichromatic Theory)
Proposed by Young, refined by Helmholtz
Three types of receptors, each with a different spectral sensitivity
Opponent-process theory was proposed by Hering.
Two different classes of cells encoding color, and another class encoding brightness
Each encodes two complementary color perceptions.
This theory accounts for color afterimages and colors that cannot appear together (reddish green or bluish yellow).
Both theories are correct: coding of color by cones seems to operate on a purely component basis; opponent processing of color is seen at all subsequent levels.
Color constancy: color perception is not altered by \n varying reflected wavelengths.
Retinex theory (Land): color is determined by the \n proportion of light of different wavelengths that a \n surface reflects.
Relative wavelengths are constant, so perception is \n constant.
Dual-opponent color cells are sensitive to color contrast.
Found in cortical “blobs”
Flow of Visual Information
Thalamic relay neurons, to
1 ̊ visual cortex (striate), to
2 ̊ visual cortex (prestriate), to
Visual association cortex
As visual information flows through hierarchy, receptive fields:
Become larger
Respond to more complex and specific stimuli
Scotomas
Areas of blindness in contralateral visual field due to damage to primary visual cortex
Detected by perimetry test \n
Completion
Patients may be unaware of scotoma; missing details are supplied by “completion.”
Blindsight
Response to visual stimuli outside conscious awareness of “seeing”
Possible explanations of blindsight
Islands of functional cells within scotoma
Direct connections between subcortical structures and secondary visual cortex; not available to conscious awareness
Neurons in each area respond to different visual cues, such as color, movement, or shape.
Lesions of each area results in specific deficits.
Anatomically distinct: about 12 functionally distinct areas have been identified so far.
Retinotopically Organized
Dorsal stream: pathway from primary visual cortex \n to dorsal prestriate cortex to posterior parietal \n cortex
The “where” pathway (location and movement), or
Pathway for the control of behavior (e.g., reaching)
Ventral stream: pathway from primary visual cortex \n to ventral prestriate cortex to inferotemporal cortex
The “what” pathway (color and shape), or
Pathway for the conscious perception of objects
Inability to Distinguish among Faces
Most prosopagnosics’ recognition deficits are not limited to faces.
Prosopagnosia is associated with damage to the ventral stream between the occipital and temporal lobes.
Prosopagnosics may be able to recognize faces in the absence of conscious awareness.
Prosopagnosics have different skin conductance responses to familiar faces compared to unfamiliar faces, even though they reported not recognizing any of the faces.
Deficiency in the Ability to See Movement
Progress in a Normal, Smooth Fashion
Can Be Induced by a High Dose of Certain Antidepressants
Associated with Damage to the Middle Temporal (MT) Area of the Cortex
Primary: input mainly from thalamic relay nuclei
For example, the striate cortex receives input from the lateral geniculate nucleus.
Secondary: input mainly from primary and secondary cortexes within the sensory system
Association: input from more than one sensory system, usually from the secondary sensory cortex
Hierarchical Organization
-→ Specificity and complexity increases with each \n level.
-→ Sensation: detecting a stimulus
-→ Perception: understanding the stimulus
Functional segregation: distinct functional \n areas within a level
Parallel processing: simultaneous analysis of \n signals along different pathways
Natural sounds are complex patterns of vibrations.
A Fourier analysis breaks natural sounds down into sine waves.
There is a complex relationship between natural sounds and perceived frequency
Sound waves enter the auditory canal of the \n ear and then cause the tympanic membrane \n (the eardrum) to vibrate.
This sets in motion the bones of the middleear—the ossicles—which trigger vibrations of the oval window.
Sound Wave > Eardrum > Ossicles \n (Hammer, Anvil, Stirrup) > Oval Window
Vibration of the oval window sets in motion the fluid of the cochlea.
The cochlea’s internal membrane—the organ of Corti—is the auditory receptor organ
The organ of Corti is composed of two membranes.
Basilar membrane: auditory receptors—hair cells \n —are mounted here.
Tectorial membrane: rests on the hair cells
Stimulation of hair cells triggers action potentials in the auditory nerve.
Cochlear Coding
Different frequencies produce maximal stimulation of hair cells at different pointsalong the basilar membrane.
The basilar membrane and most other auditory system components are organized tonotopically—that is, by frequency
The axons of each auditory nerve synapse in the ipsilateral cochlear nuclei.
From there, many projections lead to the superior \n olives on both sides of the brain stem.
From there, axons project via the lateral lemniscus \n to the inferior colliculi.
Axons then project from the inferior colliculi to the medial geniculate nuclei of the thalamus.
Thalamic neurons then project to the primary auditory cortex
The lateral and medial superior olives react to differences in what is heard by the two ears.
Medial: differences in arrival
Lateral: amplitude differences
Both project to the superior colliculus.
The deep layers of the superior colliculus are laid out \n according to auditory space, allowing location of sound \n sources in the world; the shallow layers are laid out \n retinotopically.
The auditory cortex is located in the temporal lobe.
Core region: includes primary cortex
The belt surrounds the core region
A band of secondary cortex
Areas of the secondary cortex outside the belt are referred to as parabelt areas.
About ten separate areas of secondary auditory cortex exist in primates
Functional columns: cells of a column respond to the same frequency
Tonotopic Organization
Secondary areas do not respond well to pure \n tones and have not been well researched.
There is a lack of understanding of the dimensions along which the auditory cortex evaluates sound.
All through the cortical levels of the auditory system, there are cells that respond to complex sounds.
Perhaps study with pure tones is limited
Auditory signals are conducted to two areas of association cortex.
-→ Prefrontal cortex
-→ Posterior parietal cortex
Anterior auditory pathway may be more involved in identifying sounds (what).
Posterior auditory pathway may be more involved in locating sounds (where)
There is evidence for interactions between the auditory and visual systems.
E.g., some posterior parietal neurons with both visual and auditory receptive field
Interaction in primary sensory cortices indicate that sensory system interaction is an early and integral part of sensory processing
Auditory cortex lesions in rats result in few \n permanent hearing deficits.
Lesions in monkeys and humans hinder \n sound localization and pitch discrimination.
Deafness in Humans
Total deafness is rare, due to multiple pathways.
Two kinds: conductive deafness (damage to \n ossicles) and nerve deafness (damage to cochlea)
Partial cochlear damage results in loss of hearing at \n particular frequencies
The somatosensory system is made up of three separate and interacting systems.
Exteroceptive: external stimuli
Proprioceptive: body position
Interoceptive: body conditions (e.g., temperature and blood pressure)
Free Nerve Ending
Temperature and pain
Pacinian Corpuscles
Adapt rapidly; large and deep; onion-like
Respond to sudden displacements of the skin
Merkel’s disks: gradual skin indentation
Ruffini endings: gradual skin stretch
Dermatome: the area of the body innervated by \n the left and right dorsal roots of a given segment of spinal cord
Dorsal-Column Medial-Lemniscus System
Mainly touch and proprioception
First synapse in the dorsal column nuclei of the \n medulla
Anterolateral System
Mainly pain and temperature
Synapse upon entering the spinal cord
Three tracts: spinothalamic, spinoreticular, \n spinotectal
Primary Somatosensory Cortex (SI)
Postcentral gyrus
Somatotopic organization (somatosensoryhomunculus); more sensitive, more cortex
Input largely contralateral
SII: mainly input from SI
Somatotopic; input from both sides of the body
Much of the output from SI and SII goes to \n the association cortex in the posterior \n parietal lobe.
The highest level of the sensory hierarchy is made up of areas of association cortex in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortex.
The posterior parietal cortex contains bimodal neurons.
Neurons that respond to activation of two different sensory systems
Allow integration of visual and somatosensory \n input
Astereognosia: inability to recognize objects by touch
Pure cases are rare; other sensory deficits areusually present.
Asomatognosia: the failure to recognize parts of one’s own body (e.g., the case of the man who fell out of bed)
Despite its unpleasantness, pain is adaptive and needed.
There exist no obvious cortical representation of pain (although the anterior cingulate gyrus appears to be \n involved in the emotional component of pain).
Descending pain control: pain can be suppressed by cognitive and emotional factors.
Circuitry Identified by the Following Studies:
Electrical stimulation of the periaqueductal gray (PAG) has analgesic effects.
PAG and other brain areas have opiate receptors.
Existence of Endogenous Opiates (Natural \n Analgesics); Endorphins
Neuropathic pain is severe chronic pain in the absence of a recognizable pain stimulus.
Neuropathic pain is likely the result of pathology of the nervous system linked to an injury.
Some evidence exists to suggest that aberrant microglial cell signals trigger neural pain pathways
Olfaction (Smell)
Detects airborne chemicals
Gustation (Taste)
Responds to chemicals in the mouth
Food acts on both systems to produce flavor
Pheremones are chemicals that influence that \n behavior of conspecifics (members of the same \n species).
Evidence of Human Pheromones
Changes in Olfactory Sensitivity across the \n Menstrual Cycle
Synchronization of Menstrual Cycles
Sex Identification by Smell (Especially by Women)
Men can identify a woman’s menstrual stage by smell
Receptor cells are embedded in the olfactory mucosa of the nose.
There are many different kinds of receptors.
Rats and mice have about 1,500.
Humans have almost 1,000.
Same kinds of receptor cells project to similar areas \n of the olfactory bulb.
Clusters of neurons near the surface of the olfactory bulbs
Olfactory glomeruli
New receptor cells are created throughout life.
The olfactory tract projects to several structures of the medial temporal lobes including the amygdala and the piriform cortex.
Does NOT first pass through the thalamus
Only sensory system that does this
There are receptors in the tongue and oral cavity in clusters of about 50 called taste buds.
Located around small protuberances called papillae
There are 4 (sweet, sour, salty, bitter) primary \n tastes; 5th is umami, meat or savory.
Many tastes are not created by combining \n primaries.
Salty and sour don’t have receptors; they \n merely act on ion channels
Gustatory afferent neurons leave the mouth as part of the 7th, 9th, and 10th cranial nerves to the solitary nucleus of the medulla.
Projections then pass to the ventral posterior nucleus of the thalamus.
From there, neurons project to the primary gustatory cortex and then to the secondary gustatory cortex
Anosmia: inability to smell
The most common cause is a blow to the head \n that damages olfactory nerves.
Incomplete deficits are seen with a variety of \n disorders.
Ageusia: inability to taste
Rare due to multiple pathways carrying taste \n information
There is evidence for the narrow tuning of \n gustatory receptors.
Respond to only one taste
Tuning is broader in presynaptic cells and up through the cortex
Selective attention improves perception of what is attended to and interferes with that which is not.
Internal cognitive processes (endogenous attention) and external events (exogenous attention) focus attention
The cocktail party phenomenon indicates that there is processing of information not attended to
Selective attention is thought to work by strengthening the neural responses to attended-to aspects and by weakening the responses to other.
For example, spatial attention can shift the location of receptive fields (Wommelsdorf et al., 2006)